REF 8 - Psychology
Write a reflection paper. add your own thoughts. and stories or experiences 2 page. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expectations about the visual world Patricia A. Ganea a,⇑, Allison Fitch b, Paul L. Harris c, Zsuzsa Kaldy b a University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada b University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA 02125, USA c Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Available online xxxx Keywords: Mental representation Updating Absent reference Language Object location Visual world a b s t r a c t The capacity to use language to form new representations and to revise existing knowledge is a crucial aspect of human cognition. Here we examined whether infants can use language to adjust their representation of a recently encoded scene. Using an eye- tracking paradigm, we asked whether 16-month-old infants (N = 26; mean age = 16;0 [months;days], range = 14;15–17;15) can use language about an occluded event to inform their expecta- tion about what the world will look like when the occluder is removed. We compared looking time to outcome scenes that matched the language input with looking time to those that did not. Infants looked significantly longer at the event outcome when the outcome did not match the language input, suggesting that they generated an expectation of the outcome based on that input alone. This effect was unrelated to infants’ vocabulary size. Thus, using language to adjust expectations about the visual world is present at an early developmental stage even when language skills are rudimentary. ! 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction Much of the information that we have about the world is based on testimony provided by other people. Thus, on the basis of other people’s testimony, we can acquire knowledge about events that http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.005 0022-0965/! 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. ⇑ Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (P.A. Ganea). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 we would otherwise know little or nothing about—for example, events in the distant past. In addition, however, other people’s testimony can help us to update our knowledge of the current state of the world (Harris, 2012). Indeed, we often rely on such testimony to provide us with information about changes that have occurred with respect to people or objects that are already known to us. For exam- ple, via testimony, friends and family members often provide us with updates about their lives. They can tell us about changes in their love lives, their jobs, their kitchen appliances, and so forth. By impli- cation, updating based on verbal testimony has a broad scope. Developmental research has provided a wealth of information on infants’ ability to update object representations on the basis of visual information (e.g., Feigenson & Yamaguchi, 2009; Koechlin, Dehaene, & Mehler, 1997; Uller, Carey, Huntley-Fenner, & Klatt, 1999; Wellman, Cross, & Bartsch, 1986; Wynn, 1992) such as when changes are being made to the number of objects in a scene or to an object’s location in the scene. However, only recently have we begun to learn information about children’s ability to update their knowledge of an object through language. Ganea and Harris (2010, 2013) showed that when toddlers aged 30 months were told that an object they had put in one con- tainer had been moved to a different container during their temporary absence, they searched for the object in the new container, not where they had originally put it. By implication, using the testimony provided by another person, they updated their own prior representation of the state of the world and searched correctly. However, in those same studies, younger toddlers—aged approximately 24 months—were much less accurate in making such location updates. They often searched for the object on the basis of their earlier firsthand observation of its whereabouts—an error that did not occur in a control condition where they saw the object moved to a new location rather than learning about its movement via ver- bal testimony. Alternatively, they first searched in the original container and only subsequently went on to search at the object’s new location. Thus, these younger infants were less likely than older infants to give priority to what they had been told. One possible interpretation of this age change is that younger infants have difficulty in updating their representation of the world on the basis of verbal input. To the extent that a great deal of con- versational input to young children is concerned with objects, events, and outcomes that are present and potentially observable within the immediate situation (Morford & Goldin-Meadow, 1997), such a restriction might not create any obvious cognitive difficulty. According to this hypothesis, we might expect children’s updating ability to improve as their early language becomes increasingly displaced from the here and now. However, despite the above findings, updating may emerge considerably earlier in development. There are several reasons for considering this alternative possibility. First, task demands may have underestimated the ability of younger toddlers in the studies of Ganea and Harris (2010, 2013). They may have understood what they were told and updated their representation of the object’s location but failed to inhibit a prepotent response of searching in the place where they had last seen the object. Under this interpretation, infants can engage in language-based updating but have difficulty in using the updated representation of location to guide voluntary actions such as search. Similar gaps between the representation of location and the execution of a response have been well established in studies of infants’ ability to search for a hidden object (Diamond, 1985). Second, by the beginning of the second year of life, infants are adept at incorporating testimony into their expectations about new unseen referents. For example, infants are able to reason about the quantities of unseen objects and nonobvious functions of objects through information provided via language alone (Graham, Kilbreath, & Welder, 2004; Xu, Cote, & Baker, 2005). Third, studies of infants’ ability to engage in updating with respect to an object’s properties—as opposed to its location—have yielded encouraging results. For example, in one study infants left a stuffed animal in a room and went next door. They were subsequently told that the stuffed animal had been accidentally made wet. When they went back to retrieve it, 22-month-olds picked out a wet version of the stuffed animal rather than a dry version (Ganea, Shutts, Spelke, & DeLoache, 2007). Furthermore, follow-up studies on property updating demonstrated that reducing task demands by strengthening the working memory representation of the to-be-updated object and increasing its familiarity promoted updating in even younger toddlers, with 19-month-olds succeed- ing in updating object properties (Galazka & Ganea, 2014). 2 P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 Taken together, these considerations suggest that even younger infants—provided that they can understand a simple assertion about object movement—might update their expectations about the location of an object based on what they are told. The current violation-of-expectation study tested this prediction using a computer presentation. The violation-of expectation paradigm used here was an adaptation of the blank-screen paradigm, which was originally designed to record eye move- ments as adult participants listen to a sentence when the screen is blank (Altmann & Kamide, 2004). This paradigm allows one to measure patterns of eye movements when the visual scene is absent (e.g., whether infants look to locations of mentioned absent objects) and to measure whether looks to the scene when visible are influenced by prior language. Accordingly, in this study we wanted to explore infants’ looking patterns when the visual scene was absent at the time of the linguistic input and also when the visual scene was available again. Sixteen-month-old infants were first familiarized with two pairs of common animals (two dogs and two cats) on a computer screen. Next, they were presented with a scene that included a target loca- tion—for example, a bed—and two different familiar animals, always one dog and one cat, located on either side of the target location. Infants heard a speaker call their attention to each animal and to the target location. Then children watched as a curtain went down to hide the scene and the speaker described an invisible action of one of the two animals—for example, ‘‘Now the dog goes to the table. The dog is on the table.” The curtain was then raised to reveal a scene that was either congruent with the speaker’s testimony (i.e., the dog on the table and the cat still in its original position) or incongru- ent with her testimony (i.e., the cat on the table and the dog still in its original position). If infants suc- cessfully updated their representation of the scene based on what they were told while the curtain was lowered, they should be surprised by the incongruent scene but not by the congruent scene. We tested infants’ expectations by comparing their looking time patterns to the incongruent versus congruent scene. Method Participants The participants were 41 healthy, full-term, 16-month-old infants (age range = 14;15–17;15 [months;days]). Of these participants, 11 did not complete the required two blocks of trials as a result of fussiness and were excluded from further analysis. In addition, 3 infants were excluded due to experimenter error, and 1 infant was excluded due to equipment failure. The final sample consisted of 26 infants (12 female and 14 male; average age = 16;0, SD = 27.2 days). Of this final sample, 16 infants were Caucasian (1 of them Hispanic), 3 were African American, 2 were biracial, and 5 families chose not to report their race and ethnicity. Parents of participants were recruited from the greater Boston area in the northeastern United States based on data from state birth records. They received $20 and a small gift for participation. All participants were from dominantly (>75\%) English- speaking households, with receptive vocabularies ranging from 11 to 350 words (M = 153.54, SD = 75.51) and productive vocabularies ranging from 0 to 66 words (M = 26.83, SD = 20.81) as mea- sured by the MacArthur–Bates Communication Development Inventory (MCDI). Statistics on MCDI items pertinent to the study’s design are displayed in Table 1. Apparatus We used a Tobii T120 eye-tracker running Tobii Studio 3.0 software (Tobii Technology, Stockholm, Sweden) sampling at 60 Hz to measure eye movement patterns. Eye position was determined by aver- aging data from both eyes. When data could be acquired from only one eye (e.g., when the infant’s face was momentarily not centered on the screen), they were not used. Fixations were defined using Tobii Studio’s standard built-in fixation filter (Tobii Fixation Filter), which automatically interpolates seg- ments of missing data that are shorter than 100 ms. Participants sat on their parents’ laps approxi- mately 60 cm away from the built-in 17-inch monitor in a dimly lit testing room. Parents’ eyes were shielded by a visor, by modified sunglasses with occluded lenses, or by parents closing their eyes. P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 3 Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 Stimuli and procedure We employed a within-participants design with two blocks of test trials: Congruent versus Incon- gruent trials. Each block consisted of 4 familiarization trials followed by 6 test trials. Participants saw both blocks of trials, with a short break in between for feeding, changing, or a brief walk down the hallway. (Breaks varied in length based on the individual infants’ needs but were typically between 5 and 10 min.) The order of the two blocks was counterbalanced across participants. The blocked design was chosen to maximize the likelihood of being able to observe a difference between the two conditions. At the beginning of each block, each participant was calibrated using a standard 5- point calibration procedure. Each block took approximately 2.5 min. Agents (cats and dogs) and loca- tions (table and bed) were quasi-randomly drawn from the same two sets of cats and dogs and loca- tions (table and bed) in each trial. In familiarization trials, a curtain was raised to reveal either two cats or two dogs (counterbalanced in order of presentation) for 4 s. Right after the curtain went up, a female voice (prerecorded by a native English speaker in child-directed speech) said, ‘‘Look at the cats!” or ‘‘Look at the dogs!” Test trials consisted of three phases: an initial exposure, an updating period, and an outcome period. In the initial exposure, a curtain was raised to reveal an object in the middle of the stage (either a bed or a table) and a cat and a dog on each side of the object (side counterbalanced and order quasi- randomized; see Fig. 1). The female voice said, ‘‘Look at the cat! Look at the table/bed! Look at the dog!” This initial presentation of the scene, along with the recording, lasted for 8 s. Then a curtain low- ered, occluding the scene, displaying only three identical triangles as placeholders for the positions of the animals/object. These triangles, which remained static throughout the test phase, served as anchor points for the referents and allowed participants to access each referent’s spatial location during speech processing, as has been demonstrated with adults (Altmann & Kamide, 2004; Ferreira, Apel, & Henderson, 2008). Following the descent of the curtain, the female voice said, ‘‘Now the dog goes to the table. The dog is on the table.” The curtain remained in the lowered position while the testi- mony was given for 8 s (henceforth the updating period). Then the curtain was raised again to reveal either a scene that was congruent with the statement during the updating period (the dog was on the table, whereas the cat remained in its original position; Congruent condition) or incongruent with the statement (the cat was on the table, whereas the dog remained in its original position; Incongruent condition). This outcome scene was presented for 7 s (henceforth the outcome period), and then the curtain was lowered and the trial ended. A sample video of a participant during the Incongruent condition is provided in the online Supplementary material to illustrate the procedure. Data analysis To analyze gaze patterns, we defined two rectangular (327 by 268 pixels) areas of interest (AOIs) around the two animals (the cat and the dog; see Fig. 1) during the outcome period and defined one rectangular (327 by 268 pixels) AOI around each triangle placeholder during the updating period. AOIs were defined a priori to contain the entire animal during the outcome period plus some additional space around them to allow for small calibration errors. The AOIs for the triangle placeholders during the updating period were larger, such that the adjacent AOIs filled the space between them without any gaps. Total looking time to each AOI (defined by the cumulative duration of visits within an AOI) as a proportion of looking to the entire screen was analyzed for both the updating and outcome Table 1 Summary statistics of MCDI results for names of agents and objects used in experiment. Item Understands Understands and says Does not understand Dog 10 12 2 Cat 14 7 3 Table 0 10 14 Bed 18 2 4 Note. Values in table are numbers of participants. 4 P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 periods (see Results). Trials in which a 1-s cumulative looking time threshold was not met were excluded from analysis (42 of 312 trials); participants who did not meet this threshold in at least 3 of 6 trials in both blocks were likewise excluded (11 participants, listed under excluded participants). The 1-s threshold was chosen as a conservative estimate of the minimum length of time needed for infants to process each object and its corresponding location. No other data filtering was applied. Results For each participant, a proportion of total looking time during the outcome period of each condition was calculated by dividing the total looking time to each AOI (e.g., the animal on top of the table/bed) by the total looking time to the whole screen and then averaging across the 6 trials. Proportion of look- ing time was calculated only for valid trials (at least 1 s of cumulative looking to the screen during the outcome period). All participants completed at least 3 of the 6 trials per block (M = 5.19 trials in the Congruent condition and M = 5.11 trials in the Incongruent condition), and the number of trials com- pleted did not significantly differ between conditions, t(25) = 0.278, p = .78, d = 0.05. Mean looking times during the outcome and updating periods are presented in Table 2. Outcome period To identify a violation-of-expectation (VoE) effect, we conducted a 2 (Condition) ! 2 (Order of Block Presentation) mixed design analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the proportion of looking time to the outcome AOI (the animal in the outcome location, i.e., on top of the table/bed). Data are shown Look at the cat! Look at the table! Look at the dog! Now the dog goes to the table. The dog is on the table. unmentioned mentionedcenter on top on side Fig. 1. Schematic of a test trial (Incongruent condition). Red rectangles and tags above them indicate AOIs; they were not visible to participants. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.) P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 5 Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 in Fig. 2. Results indicated a main effect of condition, F(1, 24) = 44.16, p < .001, gp2 = .648, where partic- ipants showed a significantly longer looking time in the Incongruent condition (M = .68, SD = .12) than in the Congruent condition (M = .46, SD = .11). There was no significant main effect of order, F(1, 24) = 2.02, p = .17, gp2 = .078, or interaction between condition and order, F(1, 24) = 0.02, p = .89, gp2 = .001. Proportion of looking time to the outcome AOI (the animal that was on top of the target location) was also significantly above chance in the Incongruent condition, t(25) = 7.48, p < .001, d = 1.41, and at chance in the Congruent condition, t(25) = 1.56, p = .13, d = 0.32. Finally, participants were catego- rized into those who demonstrated a VoE effect (i.e., those who looked longer in the Incongruent con- dition than in the Congruent condition) and those who did not. Of the 26 participants, 23 demonstrated a VoE effect. A chi-square goodness-of-fit test determined that this is significantly dif- ferent from chance, v2(1) = 15.385, p < .001. To explore the robustness of the VoE effect observed above, we repeated the 2 (Condition) ! 2 (Order of Block Presentation) mixed design ANOVA on proportion of looking time to the outcome AOI on just the first trial in each block.1 The effect was present from the first trial; the main effect of condition was significant, F(1, 24) = 5.143, p = .033, gp2 = .176; that is, participants looked longer at the outcome AOI in the Incongruent condition (M = .554, SD = .17) than in the Congruent condition (M = .446, SD = .20). No significant main effect of order was found, F(1, 24) = 0.53, p = .473, gp2 = .022, and condition and order did not interact, F(1, 24) = 0.11, ns, gp2 = .005. Of the 24 participants with completed MCDIs, the overwhelming majority reportedly understood the words ‘‘dog”, ‘‘cat”, and ‘‘bed”, but only 10 were reported to understand the word ‘‘table” (see Table 1). To test whether the VoE effect was dependent on the destination of the location change (i.e., only those who understood ‘‘table” showed a VoE for trials where a table was presented), trials in which the table was the destination were analyzed separately. We performed a 2 (Condition) ! 2 (Understands ‘‘Table” vs. Does Not Understand ‘‘Table”) mixed design ANOVA on proportion of looking time for these trials only. The trend from the previous analyses was present (albeit with a smaller effect) in this subset of our data; the main effect of condition was marginally significant, F(1, 22) = 4.125, p = .055, gp2 = .158, in that the proportion of looking to the outcome AOI was larger in the Incongruent condition (M = .56, SD = .13) than in the Congruent condition (M = .48, SD = .13). There was no main effect of understanding the word ‘‘table”, F(1, 22) = 0.319, p = .578, gp2 = .014, and this fac- tor did not interact with condition, F(1, 22) = 1.352, p = .257, gp2 = .058. Updating period To examine whether the differences in looking time during the outcome period were reflected in gaze patterns when the predictive expectation may have been generated, we also examined looking time differences during the updating (screen down) period. We hypothesized that if predictive expec- tations occurred, participants would display longer looking to the central AOI as well as the AOI of the mentioned animal when those referents were stated in the testimony, reflecting adult-like speech pro- cessing. Fig. 3 plots a time course of the proportion of looks to each AOI by condition during this period. We conducted a 2 (Condition) ! 2 (Order of Block Presentation) ! 3 (AOI) mixed design ANOVA on the proportion of total looking. Data are shown in Fig. 4. Results indicated a main effect of AOI, F(2, 48) = 35.59, p < .001, gp2 = .597. Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni adjusted for multiple comparisons) Table 2 Mean looking times (in seconds) to test trials by phase and condition. Condition Encoding phase Testimony phase Outcome phase Congruent 5.89 (1.75) 5.1 (1.85) 5.79 (1.10) Incongruent 5.03 (1.67) 5.19 (1.35) 5.63 (1.57) Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses. 1 We also analyzed whether looking times changed over time within a block and found no significant trends. 6 P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 demonstrated that this effect was driven by looking to the center AOI (M = .421, SD = .12), which was significantly longer than looking to both the mentioned AOI (M = .202, SD = .076, p < .001) and the unmentioned AOI (M = .188, SD = .081, p < .001). There were no significant differences between the looking times to the mentioned and unmentioned AOIs (p > .99). This analysis also yielded a significant main effect of condition, F(1, 24) = 9.26, p = .006, gp2 = .278, in that participants spent a slightly larger portion of their total looking time within the three AOIs in the Congruent condition (for any given AOI: M = .284, SD = .035) than in the Incongruent condition (for any given AOI: M = .257, SD = .035). Arguably, infants in the Incongruent condition became more aroused over time because they were presented with a conflict between sentences and outcomes on 6 successive test trials. Such a repeated violation of expectation may have led them to look around randomly during the updating (screen down) period. There was no significant main effect of order, F(1, 24) = 0.007, p = .94, gp2 < .001, and no interactions. Although participants looked overall at the center AOI more than at the other two AOIs during the updating period, we investigated the possibility that infants’ gaze behavior may have changed right around the time when they heard the agent being mentioned during the testimony. To do so, we com- pared the proportion of looking to the mentioned agent during three 500-ms time windows—pre- mention (500 ms before the first mention), post-first mention (500 ms after the first mention), and post-second mention (500 ms after the second mention)—averaged across trials within a block. We conducted a 2 (Condition) ! 3 (Time Window) repeated measures ANOVA on the proportion of total looking to the mentioned AOI. Results indicated that there was no significant main effect of condition, F(1, 19) = 0.225, p = .64, gp2 = .012, or time window, F(2, 38) = 0.926, p = .41, gp2 = .046. There was also no interaction between condition and time window, F(2, 38) = 0.942, p = .39, gp2 = .047. In the Incongruent condition, the average proportion of time spent on the mentioned agent was .257 (.215) during the pre-mention period, .286 (.314) during the first post-mention period, and .269 (.255) during the sec- ond post-mention period. In the Congruent condition, these values were .289 (.256), .262 (.258), and .184 (.168), respectively. Finally, we went on to explore possible developmental contributions to the VoE effect observed during the outcome period. To do this, we examined relationships between the magnitude of VoE 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 Congruent Incongruent Pr op or tio n of lo ok in g tim e to o ut co m e A O I Condition Congruent First Incongruent First Fig. 2. Proportions of looking time to outcome AOI (animal in the outcome location) during the outcome period as a function of condition and block order. Error bars display standard errors of the means. ***p < 0.001. P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 7 Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 (the difference in proportion of looking to the event AOI between the Incongruent and Congruent con- ditions) and age (in days) as well as receptive vocabulary size on the MCDI.2 The correlations between VoE effect magnitude and age (Pearson’s r = .06, p = .78) and receptive vocabulary (r = .27, p = .21) were not significant. Discussion The capacity to use language to acquire knowledge is a fundamental aspect of human cognition. Here we examined an important aspect of that knowledge acquisition process, namely the ability to update one’s representation of the world on the basis of verbal testimony rather than direct observa- tion. Thus, we asked whether infants can update their representation of a recently encoded visual scene based on a verbal description of a change in that scene. The visual scene contained two referent animals with an inanimate object located between them. When the visual scene was occluded, infants Fig. 3. Proportion of looks to each AOI over time during the updating period in the Congruent condition (A) and the Incongruent condition (B). Testimony and 500-ms time windows (pre-mention, post-first mention, and post-second mention) are overlaid. 2 Two children were not included in the MCDI analyses. The parent of one of these children chose not to fill out the questionnaire; another parent misunderstood the instructions, yielding an extreme score. 8 P.A. Ganea et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article in press as: Ganea, P. A., et al. Sixteen-month-olds can use language to update their expec- tations about the visual world. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.12.005 were told about the change in the scene, specifically a change in the location of one of the referent animals. When the visual scene was accessible again, infants looked longer at the event outcome (the animal on top of the inanimate …
CATEGORIES
Economics Nursing Applied Sciences Psychology Science Management Computer Science Human Resource Management Accounting Information Systems English Anatomy Operations Management Sociology Literature Education Business & Finance Marketing Engineering Statistics Biology Political Science Reading History Financial markets Philosophy Mathematics Law Criminal Architecture and Design Government Social Science World history Chemistry Humanities Business Finance Writing Programming Telecommunications Engineering Geography Physics Spanish ach e. Embedded Entrepreneurship f. Three Social Entrepreneurship Models g. Social-Founder Identity h. Micros-enterprise Development Outcomes Subset 2. Indigenous Entrepreneurship Approaches (Outside of Canada) a. Indigenous Australian Entrepreneurs Exami Calculus (people influence of  others) processes that you perceived occurs in this specific Institution Select one of the forms of stratification highlighted (focus on inter the intersectionalities  of these three) to reflect and analyze the potential ways these ( American history Pharmacology Ancient history . Also Numerical analysis Environmental science Electrical Engineering Precalculus Physiology Civil Engineering Electronic Engineering ness Horizons Algebra Geology Physical chemistry nt When considering both O lassrooms Civil Probability ions Identify a specific consumer product that you or your family have used for quite some time. This might be a branded smartphone (if you have used several versions over the years) or the court to consider in its deliberations. Locard’s exchange principle argues that during the commission of a crime Chemical Engineering Ecology aragraphs (meaning 25 sentences or more). Your assignment may be more than 5 paragraphs but not less. INSTRUCTIONS:  To access the FNU Online Library for journals and articles you can go the FNU library link here:  https://www.fnu.edu/library/ In order to n that draws upon the theoretical reading to explain and contextualize the design choices. Be sure to directly quote or paraphrase the reading ce to the vaccine. Your campaign must educate and inform the audience on the benefits but also create for safe and open dialogue. A key metric of your campaign will be the direct increase in numbers.  Key outcomes: The approach that you take must be clear Mechanical Engineering Organic chemistry Geometry nment Topic You will need to pick one topic for your project (5 pts) Literature search You will need to perform a literature search for your topic Geophysics you been involved with a company doing a redesign of business processes Communication on Customer Relations. Discuss how two-way communication on social media channels impacts businesses both positively and negatively. Provide any personal examples from your experience od pressure and hypertension via a community-wide intervention that targets the problem across the lifespan (i.e. includes all ages). Develop a community-wide intervention to reduce elevated blood pressure and hypertension in the State of Alabama that in in body of the report Conclusions References (8 References Minimum) *** Words count = 2000 words. *** In-Text Citations and References using Harvard style. *** In Task section I’ve chose (Economic issues in overseas contracting)" Electromagnetism w or quality improvement; it was just all part of good nursing care.  The goal for quality improvement is to monitor patient outcomes using statistics for comparison to standards of care for different diseases e a 1 to 2 slide Microsoft PowerPoint presentation on the different models of case management.  Include speaker notes... .....Describe three different models of case management. visual representations of information. They can include numbers SSAY ame workbook for all 3 milestones. You do not need to download a new copy for Milestones 2 or 3. When you submit Milestone 3 pages): Provide a description of an existing intervention in Canada making the appropriate buying decisions in an ethical and professional manner. Topic: Purchasing and Technology You read about blockchain ledger technology. Now do some additional research out on the Internet and share your URL with the rest of the class be aware of which features their competitors are opting to include so the product development teams can design similar or enhanced features to attract more of the market. The more unique low (The Top Health Industry Trends to Watch in 2015) to assist you with this discussion.         https://youtu.be/fRym_jyuBc0 Next year the $2.8 trillion U.S. healthcare industry will   finally begin to look and feel more like the rest of the business wo evidence-based primary care curriculum. Throughout your nurse practitioner program Vignette Understanding Gender Fluidity Providing Inclusive Quality Care Affirming Clinical Encounters Conclusion References Nurse Practitioner Knowledge Mechanics and word limit is unit as a guide only. The assessment may be re-attempted on two further occasions (maximum three attempts in total). All assessments must be resubmitted 3 days within receiving your unsatisfactory grade. You must clearly indicate “Re-su Trigonometry Article writing Other 5. June 29 After the components sending to the manufacturing house 1. In 1972 the Furman v. Georgia case resulted in a decision that would put action into motion. Furman was originally sentenced to death because of a murder he committed in Georgia but the court debated whether or not this was a violation of his 8th amend One of the first conflicts that would need to be investigated would be whether the human service professional followed the responsibility to client ethical standard.  While developing a relationship with client it is important to clarify that if danger or Ethical behavior is a critical topic in the workplace because the impact of it can make or break a business No matter which type of health care organization With a direct sale During the pandemic Computers are being used to monitor the spread of outbreaks in different areas of the world and with this record 3. Furman v. Georgia is a U.S Supreme Court case that resolves around the Eighth Amendments ban on cruel and unsual punishment in death penalty cases. The Furman v. Georgia case was based on Furman being convicted of murder in Georgia. Furman was caught i One major ethical conflict that may arise in my investigation is the Responsibility to Client in both Standard 3 and Standard 4 of the Ethical Standards for Human Service Professionals (2015).  Making sure we do not disclose information without consent ev 4. Identify two examples of real world problems that you have observed in your personal Summary & Evaluation: Reference & 188. Academic Search Ultimate Ethics We can mention at least one example of how the violation of ethical standards can be prevented. Many organizations promote ethical self-regulation by creating moral codes to help direct their business activities *DDB is used for the first three years For example The inbound logistics for William Instrument refer to purchase components from various electronic firms. During the purchase process William need to consider the quality and price of the components. In this case 4. A U.S. Supreme Court case known as Furman v. Georgia (1972) is a landmark case that involved Eighth Amendment’s ban of unusual and cruel punishment in death penalty cases (Furman v. Georgia (1972) With covid coming into place In my opinion with Not necessarily all home buyers are the same! When you choose to work with we buy ugly houses Baltimore & nationwide USA The ability to view ourselves from an unbiased perspective allows us to critically assess our personal strengths and weaknesses. This is an important step in the process of finding the right resources for our personal learning style. Ego and pride can be · By Day 1 of this week While you must form your answers to the questions below from our assigned reading material CliftonLarsonAllen LLP (2013) 5 The family dynamic is awkward at first since the most outgoing and straight forward person in the family in Linda Urien The most important benefit of my statistical analysis would be the accuracy with which I interpret the data. The greatest obstacle From a similar but larger point of view 4 In order to get the entire family to come back for another session I would suggest coming in on a day the restaurant is not open When seeking to identify a patient’s health condition After viewing the you tube videos on prayer Your paper must be at least two pages in length (not counting the title and reference pages) The word assimilate is negative to me. I believe everyone should learn about a country that they are going to live in. It doesnt mean that they have to believe that everything in America is better than where they came from. It means that they care enough Data collection Single Subject Chris is a social worker in a geriatric case management program located in a midsize Northeastern town. She has an MSW and is part of a team of case managers that likes to continuously improve on its practice. The team is currently using an I would start off with Linda on repeating her options for the child and going over what she is feeling with each option.  I would want to find out what she is afraid of.  I would avoid asking her any “why” questions because I want her to be in the here an Summarize the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psychological research (Comp 2.1) 25.0\% Summarization of the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psych Identify the type of research used in a chosen study Compose a 1 Optics effect relationship becomes more difficult—as the researcher cannot enact total control of another person even in an experimental environment. Social workers serve clients in highly complex real-world environments. Clients often implement recommended inte I think knowing more about you will allow you to be able to choose the right resources Be 4 pages in length soft MB-920 dumps review and documentation and high-quality listing pdf MB-920 braindumps also recommended and approved by Microsoft experts. The practical test g One thing you will need to do in college is learn how to find and use references. References support your ideas. College-level work must be supported by research. You are expected to do that for this paper. You will research Elaborate on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study 20.0\% Elaboration on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study is missing. Elaboration on any potenti 3 The first thing I would do in the family’s first session is develop a genogram of the family to get an idea of all the individuals who play a major role in Linda’s life. After establishing where each member is in relation to the family A Health in All Policies approach Note: The requirements outlined below correspond to the grading criteria in the scoring guide. At a minimum Chen Read Connecting Communities and Complexity: A Case Study in Creating the Conditions for Transformational Change Read Reflections on Cultural Humility Read A Basic Guide to ABCD Community Organizing Use the bolded black section and sub-section titles below to organize your paper. For each section Losinski forwarded the article on a priority basis to Mary Scott Losinksi wanted details on use of the ED at CGH. He asked the administrative resident