Evaluating Objective and Projective Asessments - Psychology
See all required articles to use attach and compare projective and objective methods of personality assessment and provide an evaluation of these techniques organized according to the outline provided below. Use information from the researched peer-reviewed articles and required sources to support each section?
Section 1: Objective Personality Assessment?
Define the term objective in objective methods of personality assessment?
Summarize the features of objective methods of personality assessment, and provide at least three examples of these types of measures?
Explain the assumptions on which objective methods are based, and provide an analysis of empirical research testing the validity of the assumptions you identified.
Appraise the research exploring the technical adequacy (i.e., reliability and validity) of objective tests?
Describe the impact of social and culture variability on the administration and interpretation of objective tests?
Section 2: Projective Personality Assessment?
Define the term projective in projective methods of personality assessment?
Summarize the features of projective methods of personality assessment, and provide at least three examples of these types of measures?
Explain the assumptions on which projective methods are based, and provide an analysis of empirical research testing the validity of the assumptions you identified?
Appraise the research exploring the technical adequacy (i.e., reliability and validity) of projective tests?
Describe the impact of social and culture variability on the administration and interpretation of projective tests?
Section 3: Synthesis, Conclusions, and Recommendations?
Brief one-paragraph scenario for a fictitious client. Include the following information: presenting concerns (reason for referral), age, gender, ethnicity, language(s), and any other significant information (e.g., military status, health issues, marital status, sexual orientation, etc.)?
Debate the arguments supporting and opposing the use of projective and objective personality assessments with your identified client?
Select a minimum of one objective and one projective measure to use with your client.?Compare the use of the selected projective and objective personality measures with your identified client?
Analyze the advantages and limitations of each assessment measure you selected?
Compose recommendations to improve the validity of personality assessment?
CHAPTER 8
Origins of Personality
Testing
TOPIC 8A Theories of Personality and
Projective Techniques
8.1 Personality: An Overview
8.2 Psychoanalytic Theories of Personality
8.3 Type Theories of Personality
8.4 Phenomenological Theories of Personality
8.5 Behavioral and Social Learning Theories
8.6 Trait Conceptions of Personality
8.7 The Projective Hypothesis
8.8 Association Techniques
8.9 Completion Techniques
8.10 Construction Techniques
8.11 Expression Techniques
Case Exhibit 8.1 Projective Tests as Ancillary
to the Interview
In psychological testing a fundamental
distinction often is drawn between ability tests
and personality tests. Defined in the broadest
sense, ability tests include a plethora of
instruments for measuring intelligence,
achievement, and aptitude. In the preceding
seven chapters we have explored the nature,
construction, application, reliability, and validity
of ability tests. In the next two chapters we shift
the emphasis to personality tests and related
matters. Personality tests seek to measure one or
more of the following: personality traits,
dynamic motivation, symptoms of distress,
personal strengths, and attitudinal
characteristics. Measures of spirituality,
creativity, and emotional intelligence also fall
within this realm.
Theories of personality provide an underpinning
for the multiplicity of instruments available in
the field. For this reason, we begin this chapter
with a survey of prominent personality theories.
The many ways in which theorists conceptualize
personality clearly have impacted the design of
personality tests and assessments. This is
especially evident with projective techniques
such as the Rorschach inkblot method, which
emanated from psychoanalytic conceptions of
personality. Thus, in Topic 8A, Theories of
Personality and Projective Techniques, in
addition to the survey of personality theories,
we have included an introduction to several
instruments based on the turn-of-the-twentieth-
century psychoanalytic hypothesis where
responses to ambiguous stimuli reveal the
innermost, unconscious mental processes of the
examinee. The coverage of personality
assessment continues in Topic 8B, Self-Report
and Behavioral Assessment of Psychopathology,
which includes a review of structured tests and
procedures, including self-report inventories and
behavioral assessment approaches. These time-
honored topics of Chapter 8—theories of
personality, projective techniques, and
structured personality tests—are followed by the
relatively new focus of Chapter 9—the
assessment of normality and human strengths.
8.1 PERSONALITY: AN
OVERVIEW
Although personality is difficult to define, we
can distinguish two fundamental features of this
vague construct. First, each person is consistent
to some extent; we have coherent traits and
action patterns that arise repeatedly. Second,
each person is distinctive to some extent;
behavioral differences exist between
individuals. Consider the reactions of three
graduate students when their midterm
examinations were handed back. Although all
three students received nearly identical grades
(solid B’s), personal reactions were quite
diverse. The first student walked off sullenly
and was later overheard to say that a complaint
to the departmental administrator was in order.
The second student was pleased, stating out loud
that a B was, after all, a respectable grade. The
third student was disappointed but stoical. He
blamed himself for not studying harder.
How are we to understand the different
reactions of these three persons, each of whom
was responding to an identical stimulus?
Psychologists and laypersons alike invoke the
concept of personality to make sense out of the
behavior and expressed feelings of others. The
notion of personality is used to explain
behavioral differences between persons (for
example, why one complains and another is
stoical) and to understand the behavioral
consistency within each individual (for example,
why the complaining student noted previously
was generally sour and dissatisfied).
Why people differ is just one of many key
issues in the study of personality. Mayer (2007–
8) provides a thoughtful discussion of the big
questions in personality psychology, which he
defines as “those questions that are simple,
important, and central to many people’s lives.”
He identifies 20 big questions, only a few of
which can be addressed through testing and
assessment. These questions involve existential
matters such as the purpose of life, the nature of
personhood, and the difficulties encountered in
seeking self-knowledge. His captivating article
is a reminder that some vital issues can be
approached through the empiricism of
psychological research and testing, whereas
other crucial matters remain elusive and are
amenable mainly to philosophical and
phenomenological inquiry.
In addition to understanding personality,
psychologists also seek to measure it. Literally
hundreds of personality tests are available for
this purpose; we will review historically
prominent instruments and also discuss some
promising new approaches. However, in order
that the reader can better comprehend the
diversity of instruments and approaches, we
begin with a more fundamental question: How
is personality best conceptualized? As the reader
will discover, in order to measure personality
we must first envision what it is we seek to
measure. The reader will better appreciate the
multiplicity of tests and procedures if we also
briefly describe the personality theories that
comprise the underpinnings for these
instruments.
8.2 PSYCHOANALYTIC
THEORIES OF
PERSONALITY
Psychoanalysis was the original creation of
Sigmund Freud (1856–1939). While it is true
that many others have revised and adapted his
theories, the changes have been slight in
comparison to the substantial foundations that
can be traced to this singular genius of the
Victorian and early-twentieth-century era. Freud
was enormously prolific in his writing and
theorizing. We restrict our discussion to just
those aspects of psychoanalysis that have
influenced psychological testing. In particular,
the Rorschach, the Thematic Apperception Test,
and most of the projective techniques critiqued
in the next topic dictate a psychoanalytic
framework for interpretation. Readers who wish
a more thorough review of Freud’s contributions
can start with the New Introductory Lectures on
Psychoanalysis (Freud, 1933). Reviews and
interpretations of Freud’s theories can be found
in Stafford-Clark (1971) and Fisher and
Greenberg (1984).
Origins of Psychoanalytic Theory
Freud began his professional career as a
neurologist but was soon specializing in the
treatment of hysteria, an emotional disorder
characterized by histrionic behavior and
physical symptoms of psychic origin such as
paralysis, blindness, and loss of sensation. With
his colleague Joseph Breuer, Freud postulated
that the root cause of hysteria was buried
memories of traumatic experiences such as
childhood sexual molestation. If these memories
could be brought forth under hypnosis, a release
of emotion called abreaction would take place
and the hysterical symptoms would disappear, at
least briefly (Studies on Hysteria, Breuer &
Freud, 1893–1895).
From these early studies Freud developed a
general theory of psychological functioning
with the concept of the unconscious as its
foundation. He believed that the unconscious
was the reservoir of instinctual drives and a
storehouse of thoughts and wishes that would be
unacceptable to our conscious self. Thus, Freud
argued that our most significant personal
motivations are largely beyond conscious
awareness. The concept of the unconscious was
discussed in elaborate detail in his first book
(The Interpretation of Dreams, Freud, 1900).
Freud believed that dreams portray our
unconscious motives in a disguised form. Even
a seemingly innocuous dream might actually
have a hidden sexual or aggressive meaning, if
it is interpreted correctly.
Freud’s concept of the unconscious penetrated
the very underpinnings of psychological testing
early in the twentieth century. An entire family
of projective techniques emerged, including ink-
blot tests, word association approaches,
sentence completion techniques, and storytelling
(apperception) techniques (Frank, 1939, 1948).
Each of these methods was predicated on the
assumption that unconscious motives could be
divined from an examinee’s responses to
ambiguous and unstructured stimuli. In fact,
Rorschach (1921) likened his inkblot test to an
X ray of the unconscious mind. Although he
patently overstated the power of projective
techniques, it is evident from Rorschach’s view
that the psychoanalytic conception of the
unconscious had a strong influence on testing
practices.
The Structure of the Mind
Freud divided the mind into three structures: the
id, the ego, and the superego. The id is the
obscure and inaccessible part of our personality
that Freud likened to “a chaos, a cauldron of
seething excitement.” Because the id is entirely
unconscious, we must infer its characteristics
indirectly by analyzing dreams and symptoms
such as anxiety. From such an analysis, Freud
concluded that the id is the seat of all instinctual
needs such as for food, water, sexual
gratification, and avoidance of pain. The id has
only one purpose, to obtain immediate
satisfaction for these needs in accordance with
the pleasure principle. The pleasure principle is
the impulsion toward immediate satisfaction
without regard for values, good or evil, or
morality. The id is also incapable of logic and
possesses no concept of time. The chaotic
mental processes of the id are, therefore,
unaltered by the passage of time, and
impressions that have been pushed down into
the id “are virtually immortal and are preserved
for whole decades as though they had only
recently occurred” (Freud, 1933).
If our personality consisted only of an id
striving to gratify its instincts without regard for
reality, we would soon be annihilated by outside
forces. Fortunately, soon after birth, part of the
id develops into the ego or conscious self. The
purpose of the ego is to mediate between the id
and reality. The ego is part of the id and servant
to it, but the ego “interpolates between desire
and action the procrastinating factor of thought”
(Freud, 1933). Thus, the ego is largely
conscious and obeys the reality principle; it
seeks realistic and safe ways of discharging the
instinctual tensions that are constantly pushing
forth from the id.
The ego must also contend with the superego,
the ethical component of personality that starts
to emerge in the first five years of life. The
superego is roughly synonymous with
conscience and comprises the societal standards
of right and wrong that are conveyed to us by
our parents. The superego is partly conscious,
but a large part of it is unconscious, that is, we
are not always aware of its existence or
operation. The function of the superego is to
restrict the attempts of the id and ego to obtain
gratification. Its main weapon is guilt, which it
uses to punish the wrongdoings of the ego and
id. Thus, it is not enough for the ego to find a
safe and realistic way for the gratification of id
strivings. The ego must also choose a morally
acceptable outlet, or it will suffer punishment
from its overseer, the superego. This explains
why we may feel guilty for immoral behavior
such as theft even when getting caught is
impossible. Another part of the superego is the
ego ideal, which consists of our aims and
aspirations. The ego measures itself against the
ego ideal and strives to fulfill its demands for
perfection. If the ego falls too far short of
meeting the standards of the ego ideal, a feeling
of guilt may result. We commonly interpret this
feeling as a sense of inferiority (Freud, 1933).
The Role of Defense Mechanisms
The ego certainly has a difficult task, acting as
mediator and servant to three tyrants: id,
superego, and external reality. It may seem to
the reader that the task would be essentially
impossible and that the individual would,
therefore, be in a constant state of anxiety.
Fortunately, the ego has a set of tools at its
disposal to help carry out its work, namely,
mental strategies collectively labeled defense
mechanisms.
Defense mechanisms come in many varieties,
but they all share three characteristics in
common. First, their exclusive purpose is to
help the ego reduce anxiety created by the
conflicting demands of id, superego, and
external reality. In fact, Freud felt that anxiety
was a signal telling the ego to invoke one or
more defense mechanisms in its own behalf.
Defense mechanisms and anxiety are, therefore,
complementary concepts in psychoanalytic
theory, one existing as a counterforce to the
other. The second common feature of defense
mechanisms is that they operate unconsciously.
Thus, even though defense mechanisms are
controlled by the ego, we are not aware of their
operation. The third characteristic of defense
mechanisms is that they distort inner or outer
reality. This property is what makes them
capable of reducing anxiety. By allowing the
ego to view a challenge from the id, superego,
or external reality in a less-threatening manner,
defense mechanisms help the ego avoid
crippling levels of anxiety. Of course, because
they distort reality, the rigid, excessive
application of defense mechanisms may create
more problems than it solves.
Assessment of Defense Mechanisms and
Ego Functions
Although Freud introduced the concept of
defense mechanisms, it was left to his followers
to elucidate these unconscious mental strategies
in more detail (Paulhus, Fridhandler, & Hayes,
1997). Vaillant (1971) developed a hierarchy of
ego defense mechanisms based on the
assumption that some mechanisms are healthier
or more adaptive than others. He suggested four
broad types, listed here in ascending level of
maturity: psychotic, immature, neurotic, mature.
Each type includes specific defense mechanisms
such as denial, projection, repression, and
altruism, described below. Perry and Henry
(2004) proposed a similar hierarchy of
adaptation in defense mechanisms. They also
developed a sophisticated rating scale, which, as
we will see, is of value in clinical practice. A
hierarchy of types of defense mechanisms (least
mature to most mature) is provided in Table 8.1.
Psychotic defense mechanisms are the least
healthy because they distort reality to an
extreme degree. One example includes gross
denial of external reality such as the refusal to
acknowledge the death of a loved one. Another
example is delusional projection, which consists
of frank delusions about external reality, usually
of a persecutory nature. The second grouping,
Acting Out, comprises several forms of
maladaptive action such as passive-aggressive
behavior (e.g., intentional lateness to aggravate
a partner), impulsive behavior designed to
reduce tension, and complaining while
simultaneously rejecting help.
Borderline defense mechanisms include patterns
of behavior often found in persons with a
diagnosis of Borderline Personality Disorder
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The
specific mechanisms include splitting, in which
the images of others (or self) alternate rapidly
from all good to all bad, and projective
identification which is the projection of an
unwanted, unrecognized trait (like anger) onto
others. Neurotic defense mechanisms, the fourth
group, are found to some degree in most persons
and include repression (inexplicable memory
lapses or failure to acknowledge information,
such as “forgetting” a dental appointment) and
displacement, which comprises the transfer of
feelings from the real object onto someone or
something else, such as kicking the dog when
angry with the boss.
TABLE 8.1 A Hierarchy of Types of Defense
Mechanisms (Least Mature to Most Mature)
Source: Based on Perry and Henry (2004) and Vaillant
(1977).
Obsessive defense mechanisms also are very
common and consist of mental patterns like
isolation of affect or intellectualization.
Isolation of affect involves the superficial
Type Description and Examples
Psyc
hotic
Gross denial of external reality such as
frank delusions; includes denial and
distortion
Acti
ng
Out
Maladaptive behaviors such as impulsive
actions; includes passive-aggressiveness
Bord
erlin
e
Splitting the image of others into good
and bad; includes splitting and schizoid
fantasy
Neur
otic
Mechanisms that involve minor reality
distortion; includes repression and
displacement
Obse
ssive
Somewhat adaptive mechanisms; includes
isolation of affect and intellectualization
Matu
re
Mature forms of defense with minor
reality distortion; includes humor and
sublimation
acknowledgement of a feeling in the absence of
a full emotional experience. In
intellectualization, threatening matters are
acknowledged but explored in bland terms that
are relatively devoid of feelings. For example,
Vaillant (1971) describes a physician whose
mother had died recently of cancer. The doctor
talked at length about the medical characteristics
of her illness, thereby easing his sense of loss.
Mature defense mechanisms appear to the
beholder as convenient virtues. An example is
certain forms of humor that do not distort reality
but that can ease the burden of matters “too
terrible to be borne” (Vaillant, 1977). Specific
kinds of mature mechanisms include:
• Altruism: Vicarious but constructive and
gratifying service to others.
• Humor: Playful acknowledgment of ideas
and feelings without discomfort and without
unpleasant effects on others; does not
include sarcasm.
• Suppression: Conscious or semiconscious
decision to postpone paying attention to a
conscious conflict or impulse.
• Anticipation: Realistic anticipation of or
planning for future inner discomfort; for
example, realistic anticipation of surgery or
separation.
• Sublimation: Indirect expression of
instinctual wishes without adverse
consequences or loss of pleasure; for
example, channeling aggression into sports.
An example of humor as a mature defense
mechanism would be former president Ronald
Reagan’s quip to doctors in 1981 as he entered
surgery for a bullet wound from his attempted
assassination. He is reported to have said, “I
hope you’re all Republicans.”
Perry and colleagues developed the Defense
Mechanism Rating Scales (DMRS) as a basis
for assessing the level, type, and severity of
defense mechanisms encountered in
psychotherapy patients (Perry, 1990; Perry &
Harris, 2004). The DMRS was devised for
rating the presence of 30 discrete defense
mechanisms (e.g., acting out, splitting, denial,
projection, repression, intellectualization,
altruism, etc.) in a 50-minute dynamically
oriented interview. In the original scale, a 3-
point qualitative rating of absent, probably
present, or definitely present was obtained for
each defense mechanism identified in a review
of a videotaped session.
Subsequently, the test developers adopted a
simple quantitative scoring approach in which
defense mechanisms were isolated and
identified in short, meaningful segments of the
taped interview. They found that a typical
therapy session includes anywhere from 15 to
75 illustrations of the various defense
mechanisms. Based on prior research, each
defense mechanism receives a score from 1
(highly immature and maladaptive) to 7 (highly
mature and adaptive). Although the scale offers
a number of scoring options, the most useful
score is the Overall Defensive Functioning
(ODF) score, which is the simple average of the
ratings of the observed defense mechanisms.
The theoretical range of scores is 1.0 to 7.0,
although scores of 3.0 and below are rare.
Scores below 5.0 indicate significant personality
disorder or severe depression. Scores of 6.0 and
higher indicate normal or healthy functioning.
Interrater reliabilities from six studies were
mostly in the mid- to high-.80s for the ODF
scores. The stability coefficient for a small
sample of patients over a one-month interval
was a respectable .75 (Perry & Harris, 2004).
The ODF scores tend to improve over the course
of dynamically oriented therapy, which supports
the validity of the construct being measured,
maturity of defense mechanisms. In four studies
involving one-month to one-year follow-up with
small samples, the within-group effect sizes for
gains in ODF scores ranged from .02 to 1.05,
with most in the range of .41 to .82 (Perry
Harris, 2004, Table 9.5). Effect sizes of this
magnitude are considered moderate to large,
that is, meaningful gains are being
accomplished, as registered by the increased
maturity of the defense mechanisms emerging in
the therapy sessions. The authors observe:
Defenses can be viewed as both process
phenomena (psychological mechanisms in
action) and as a measure of adaptive
outcome, when aggregated across sessions
and time. This gives the study of defenses
great potential clinical relevance. To
develop and test predictive hypotheses
about treatment will make the study of
defense very relevant to daily clinical
work, and both scientifically promising and
exciting (Perry & Harris, 2004, p. 190).
The meaningful assessment of defense
mechanisms largely has eluded clinical
researchers, but instruments like the DMRS
show promise of making key elements of
psychoanalytic theory accessible to empirical
validation (Perry, Beck, Constantinides, &
Foley, 2009). However, this approach does have
two drawbacks: The practitioner needs
specialized training to identify defense
mechanisms, and the process of collecting
relevant information from patients is very time-
consuming.
8.3 TYPE THEORIES OF
PERSONALITY
The earliest personality theories attempted to
sort individuals into discrete categories or types.
For example, the Greek physician Hippocrates
(ca. 460–377 B.C.) proposed a humoral theory
with four personality types (sanguine, choleric,
melancholic, and phlegmatic) that was too
simplistic to be useful. In the 1940s, Sheldon
and Stevens (1942) proposed a type theory
based on the relationship between body build
and temperament. Their approach stimulated a
flurry of research and then faded into obscurity.
Nonetheless, typological theories have
continued to capture intermittent interest among
personality researchers. We will illustrate type
theories by reviewing contemporary research on
coronary-prone personality types.
Type A Coronary-Prone Behavior
Pattern
Friedman and Rosenman (1974) investigated the
psychological variables that put individuals at
higher risk of coronary heart disease. They were
the first to identify a Type A coronary-prone
behavior pattern, which they described as “an
action–emotion complex that can be observed in
any person who is aggressively involved in a
chronic, incessant struggle to achieve more and
more in less and less time, and if required to do
so, against the opposing efforts of other things
or persons” (Friedman & Rosenman, 1974). At
the opposite extreme is the Type B behavior
pattern, characterized by an easygoing, non-
competitive, relaxed lifestyle. Of course, people
vary along a continuum from “pure” Type A to
“pure” Type B.
Friedman and Ulmer (1984) have provided a
detailed description of the full-fledged Type A
behavior pattern, and it is not an appealing
picture. These individuals display a deep
insecurity, regardless of their achievements.
They desire to dominate others, and typically
are indifferent to the feelings of competitors.
They exhibit a free-floating hostility, and easily
find things that irritate them. They also suffer
from a sense of urgency about getting things
done. Type A persons often engage in
multitasking, such as reviewing correspondence
while making a phone call. Almost beyond
belief, one patient confessed to using two
electric shavers, one for each hand (Friedman &
Ulmer, 1984).
In other studies, researchers have found only a
weak relationship—or no relationship at all—
between Type A behavior and CHD (e.g., Eaker
& Castelli, 1988; Smedslund & Rundmo, 1999).
In the most comprehensive review of its kind,
Myrtek (2007) conducted a meta-analysis of 25
prospective studies of Type A behavior and
CHD and concluded flatly that “Type A
behavior is not an independent risk factor for
CHD.” Effect sizes in this review were not just
small, they were effectively zero, on the order of
.003. It did not matter whether structured
interviews or questionnaires were used to assess
Type A behavior. Myrtek (2007) also warns that
the existence of the concept itself can be
dangerous because it provides patients an
“external causal attribution” and relieves them
of the responsibility for behavior change. The
Type A concept also gives false benefit to
physicians when they work with CHD patients
who lack the usual risk factors (smoking, poor
diet, lack of exercise). Blaming Type A behavior
is easier than admitting that the causes of CHD
sometimes are unknown.
Other researchers have found that CHD is
linked not so much with the full-blown Type A
behavior pattern as with specific components
such as being anger-prone (Dembroski,
MacDougall, Williams, & Haney, 1985) or
possessing time urgency (Wright, 1988).
Wielgosz and Nolan (2000) identified hostility,
cynicism, and suppression of anger, as well as
stress, depression, and social isolation as
significant risk factors in Type A behavior.
Certainly there continues to be a need to sort out
the specific risk factors in this area of
investigation. What we do know with certainty
is that the simple equation of Type A behavior
causes CHD no longer is convincing.
Type A behavior can be diagnosed from a short
interview consisting of questions about habits of
working, talking, eating, reading, and thinking
(Friedman, 1996). The more flagrant cases of
Type A behavior can also be detected by paper-
and-pencil tests (Jackson & Gray, 1987).
However, the questionnaire approach is limited
because it cannot reveal the facial, vocal, and
psychomotor indices of hostility and time
urgency that are usually evident in interview
(Friedman & Ulmer, 1984).
Early studies indicated that persons who
exhibited the Type A behavior pattern were at
greatly increased risk of coronary disease and
heart attack. In one 9-year study of more than
3,000 healthy men, persons with the Type A
behavior pattern were 2½times more likely to
suffer heart attacks than those with Type B
behavior pattern (Friedman & Ulmer, 1984). In
fact, not one of the “pure” Type B’s—the
extremely relaxed, easygoing, and
noncompetitive members of the study—had
suffered a heart attack. In the famous
Framingham longitudinal study, Type A men
ages 55 to 64 were about twice as likely at 10-
year follow-up to develop coronary heart
disease as Type B men (Haynes, Feinleib, &
Eaker, 1983). In this study, the link between
Type A behavior and coronary heart disease
(CHD) was especially strong for white-collar
workers.
8.4 PHENOMENOLOGICAL
THEORIES OF
PERSONALITY
Phenomenological theories of personality
emphasize the importance of immediate,
personal, subjective experience as a determinant
of behavior. Some of the theoretical positions
subsumed under this title have been given other
labels also, such as humanistic theories,
existential theories, construct theories, self-
theories, and fulfillment theories (Maddi, 2000).
Nonetheless, these approaches share a common
focus on the person’s subjective experience,
personal world view, and self-concept as the
major wellsprings of behavior.
Origins of the Phenomenological
Approach
The orientation briefly reviewed in this section
has numerous sources that reach back to turn-of-
the-twentieth-century European philosophy and
literature. Nonetheless, two persons, one a
philosopher and the other a writer, stand out as
seminal contributors to the modern
phenomenological viewpoint. The German
philosopher Edmund Husserl (1859–1938)
invented a complex philosophy of
phenomenology that was concerned with the
description of pure mental phenomena.
Husserl’s approach was heavily introspective
and nearly inscrutable. More approachable was
the Danish writer Søren Kierkegaard (1813–
1855), well known for his contributions to
existentialism. Existentialism is the literary and
philosophical movement concerned with the
meaning of life and an individual’s freedom to
choose personal goals. The phenomenology of
Husserl and the existentialism of Kierkegaard
influenced dozens of prominent philosophers
and psychologists. Vestiges of these early
viewpoints are evident in virtually every
contemporary phenomenological personality
theory (Maddi, 2000).
Carl Rogers, Self-Theory, and the Q-
Technique
The most influential phenomenological theorist
was Carl Rogers (1902–1987). His contributions
to personality theory, known as self-theory, are
extensive and …
CHAPTER 9
Assessment of Normality
and Human Strengths
TOPIC 9A Assessment Within the
Normal Spectrum
9.1 Broad Band Tests of Normal Personality
9.2 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI)
9.3 California Psychological Inventory (CPI)
9.4 NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO-
PI-R)
9.5 Stability and Change in Personality
9.6 Assessment of Moral Judgment
9.7 Assessment of Spiritual and Religious
Concepts
In the previous chapter we surveyed tests used
by psychologists to evaluate clients for a range
of symptoms and life difficulties. These
instruments included the mainstays of the
profession such as the MMPI-2, MCMI-III,
Rorschach, and TAT. Such tests might be
referred to as “clinical” in nature, because they
are well suited to the needs of clinical practice.
But what are practitioners to do if they want to
evaluate someone who is reasonably normal? In
other words, assessment does not always entail
delving into symptoms, distress level, defense
mechanisms, diagnosis, and the like. One
example might be a young executive who wants
to know about “growth edges” in regard to
leadership positions. Another example might be
a college student who desires self-knowledge as
part of vocational explorations.
Even though clinical tests such as those
surveyed in the previous chapter can be
employed within the normal spectrum, they do
not excel in this application. In fact, the
evaluation of normal personality was not the
original purpose of tests such as the MMPI or
the Rorschach. For example, the initial objective
of the MMPI-2 was the diagnosis of
psychopathology, which remains the most
dominant and effective application of the
instrument. Historically, the purpose of the
Rorschach has been described by Frank (1939)
and others as providing an “X-ray of the mind”
to identify themes hidden away from ordinary
observation. Currently, the most common
application of the test is with clients who
display complex psychological symptoms that
do not fit neatly into the categories of the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV).
When a practitioner wants to assess personality
within the normal spectrum, tests designed
expressly for that purpose typically provide a
more helpful perspective than instruments
developed from the standpoint of
psychopathology. Instead of measuring concepts
such as depression, paranoia, anxiety,
narcissism, or suicide potential, the focus in
these alternative instruments is on qualities
pertinent to the normal range of human
functioning. We are referring here to features
like responsibility, social presence, intuition,
locus of control, attachment style, or faith
maturity. This chapter investigates an
assortment of instruments suitable for
assessment within the normal continuum and
beyond.
Normality differs from abnormality by shades of
gray rather than revealing a sharp demarcation
(Offer & Sabshin, 1966). Understanding the
various definitions of normality would involve a
lengthy detour; we do not pursue the topic here.
In their comprehensive textbook of psychiatry,
Sadock and Sadock (2004) provide an excellent
overview. Our goal here is to focus on useful
tests and measures, including some that have
been neglected because of the emphasis on
psychopathology within the field of clinical
psychology.
In Topic 9A, Assessment Within the Normal
Spectrum, we explore the qualities of several
tests and discuss their strengths and weaknesses.
We feature a few widely used scales in this
topic, including the venerable Myers-Briggs
Type Indicator (Myers & McCaulley, 1985), one
of the most widely employed personality tests of
all time, and the California Psychological
Inventory (Gough & Bradley, 1996), a measure
with strong empirical roots.
In addition to their value in the assessment of
client personality, tests also contribute to our
understanding of both typical and atypical
trajectories of personality across the life span.
For this reason, we follow a key research issue
in personality psychology, namely, whether
personality remains stable or tends to shift in
specific directions with age. We close the topic
with an evaluation of tools for assessing
spiritual and religious constructs.
Other forms of assessment pertinent to the
normal spectrum of adult functioning also are
covered in Topic 9A. We are referring here to
the evaluation of spiritual, religious, and moral
constructs. These specialized forms of
assessment have received an increasing amount
of attention in recent years.
In Topic 9B, Positive Psychological Assessment,
we examine a number of relatively new scales
that have emerged in response to a reawakening
of interest in human potential, an interest that
has remained largely dormant in psychology
since the early 1900s (Seligman &
Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). A special focus in this
topic is the assessment of creativity.
9.1 BROAD BAND TESTS OF
NORMAL PERSONALITY
A broad band test is one that measures the full
range of functioning, as opposed to limited
aspects. Beginning in the 1940s, researchers
sought to capture the nuances of normal
personality by developing broad-band self-
report instruments. The sheer variety of
approaches to this task is a testament to the
complexity of human functioning. An enduring
question, related to the previous topic on
theories of personality, is how best to
conceptualize the multi-faceted notion of
personality. For example, is personality best
construed as a limited number of types, with
most people resembling one type or another
with reasonable precision? Or, is personality
best interpreted as several dimensions, with
each unique individual revealing a specific level
of each dimension? If a dimensional approach is
preferred, how many dimensions are needed to
describe the array of human responses: 5, 16, 20
—or more?
There are no definitive answers to these
questions, although dimensional approaches
generally have prevailed over typological
methods in the history of test development.
Even so, useful and popular typological
approaches do exist. In fact, we begin the
discussion of broad-band tests with an
instrument that flexibly permits both a
typological and a dimensional approach to the
understanding of normal personality.
9.2 MYERS-BRIGGS TYPE
INDICATOR (MBTI)
Originally published in 1962, the MBTI is a
forced-choice, self-report inventory that
attempts to classify persons according to an
adaptation of Carl Jung’s theory of personality
types (Myers & McCaulley, 1985; Tzeng, Ware,
& Chen, 1989). As discussed below, recent
adaptations of the test also provide dimensional
scores in addition to the well-known four-letter
typological codes.
According to the publisher, the MBTI is the
most widely used individual test in history,
taken by approximately 2 million people a year.
Proponents of the instrument deem it valuable in
vocational guidance and organizational
consulting. It comes in a number of versions,
including Form M, a 93-item test which can be
purchased by qualified psychologists in a self-
scoring paper-and-pencil format, or
administered on-line. Other forms such as the
126-item Form G and the 144-item Form Q are
available on-line and must be authorized by a
psychologist who has agreed to a licensing
arrangement with the publisher, Consulting
Psychologists Press (www.cpp.com).
Regardless of the version employed, the MBTI
is scored on four theoretically independent
polarities: Extraversion–Introversion, Sensing–
iNtuition, Thinking–Feeling, and Judging–
Perceiving. The test-taker is categorized on one
side or the other of each polarity, which results
in a four-letter code such as ENTJ
(Extraversion, iNtuition, Thinking, Judging).
Because there are two poles to each of the four
http://www.cpp.com/
dimensions, this allows for 24 or 16 different
personality types. Each of the 16 types has been
studied extensively over the years.
The four polarities (E-I, S-N, T-F, J-P) do not
necessarily correspond to common
understandings of the anchor terms and hence
require some explanation. It is also important to
note that the concepts are intended to be value-
neutral and merely descriptive. Thus, it is
neither better nor worse to manifest
Extraversion or Introversion. Likewise,
Thinking and Feeling are simply different
modalities and one is not better than the other,
and so forth. The opposite ends of each polarity
are simply different modes of being that may
have a variety of implications for relationships,
vocation, leadership, and personal functioning.
Possessing the qualities of one polarity or the
other may be advantageous (or not) in different
situations.
Extraversion–Introversion is probably the
easiest to describe. An extravert (E) directs
energy outward to people and conversations,
whereas an introvert (I) directs energy inward to
his or her inner world. A note of clarification:
The MBTI retains the original spelling of
Extraversion, preferred by Jung, instead of using
the synonymous concept of Extroversion,
preferred by contemporary psychologists.
Sensing–iNtuition involves two opposite ways
of perceiving. Those who prefer sensing (S) rely
on the immediate senses, whereas those who
prefer intuition (N) rely upon “relationships and/
or possibilities that have been worked out
beyond the reach of the conscious mind” (Myers
& McCaulley, 1985). Of course, the letter N is
used to designate intuition because the letter I
already is taken to label Introversion. Thinking–
Feeling refers to basing conclusions on thinking
(T), that is, logic and objectivity, as opposed to
feeling (F), which involves a reliance on
personal values and social harmony. Finally,
Judging–Perceiving indicates a preference for
decisiveness and closure (J) or an open-ended
flexibility and spontaneity (P). Whereas in
common parlance the notion of “judging” often
has a negative connotation, this is not the case
when the term is applied to this polarity of the
MBTI.
The 16 possible four-letter types are not equally
represented in the general population, and some
types are more common in specific occupational
groups. For example, in a sample of 231
education graduate students from a Midwestern
university, the ENFP type was by far the most
common (N = 43), followed by ENFJ (N = 28)
in frequency. Codes beginning with the letter E
(Extraversion) constituted nearly two-thirds of
this sample, which highlights the importance of
Extraversion in the field of education.
Paraphrasing from Myers and McCaulley (1985,
p. 78), the work expectations for someone who
embodies the ENFP type are as follows:
• prefers to work interactively with a
succession of people away from the desk
• likes to work with a succession of new
problems to be solved
• prefers to provide service that is appreciated
• likes to work in changing situations that
require adaptation
These qualities align well with the role
expectations for people heading into the field of
education.
Standardization data for the MBTI is extensive
and based on large samples collected over many
decades (Myers & McCaulley, 1985). One
particularly useful table is a list of occupations
empirically attractive to the sixteen types. For
example, 18 percent of attorneys are INTJ in
type, whereas only 2 percent of elementary
school teachers fit this code. This is useful
information for clients who take the test in
search of personal or career guidance. Split-half
reliabilities for the four scales are in the .80s for
the combined subject pool of nearly 56,000
participants. Test–retest reliabilities for the four
scales are somewhat lower and depend on the
interval between tests. When the interval is
short, on the order of a few weeks, results are
strong, with coefficients mainly in the .70s and
higher. Yet, when the interval is longer, on the
order of several years, the coefficients are
predictably lower, in the .40s and .50s. With
regard to reliability, an important question with
the MBTI is the stability of the four letter code
from test to retest. The test manual reports on a
dozen studies of code type stability, with retest
intervals ranging from 5 weeks to 5 years (most
intervals a year or two). On average, about 41
percent of examinees retained their identical
code type, that is, all four letters of the code
remained the same from test to retest. About 38
percent of examinees remained stable on three
of the four letters, that is, one letter changed for
them. About 17 percent of examinees retained
two of their four letters, but switched on the
other two. And, 3 percent retained only one
letter, switching on the other three. Overall,
these are impressive results as to the long-term
stability of the MBTI code types.
In a review of 17 studies reporting reliability
coefficients, Capraro and Capraro (2002) found
respectably strong reliability coefficients of .84
(E-I), .84 (S-N), .67 (T-F), and .82 (J-P). Salter,
Forney, and Evans (2005) conducted an
especially rigorous evaluation of MBTI
reliability, looking at the stability of MBTI
categories across three administrations with 231
graduate students in education. The three
administrations were at the beginning of the first
year, beginning of the second year, and end of
the second year. Their report included extensive
analyses, but of interest here is the percentage of
respondents who received the same
classification (e.g., Extraversion or Introversion)
on all three occasions. The percentage who
displayed complete consistency for each
dimension was as follows:
Given the stringency of the reliability approach
(agreement across three administrations), these
are respectable findings.
More than 400 references citing the MBTI were
found in PsychINFO from 2000 to 2009, many
pertaining to the validity of the instrument. For
example, in a study of 177 managers, Higgs
(2001) reported a significant relationship
between emotional intelligence and the
dominant MBTI function of iNtuition.
• E-I 67\%
• S-N 66\%
• T-F 69\%
• J-P 71\%
Emotional intelligence is monitoring emotions
of self and others and using this information to
guide thinking and actions (Mayer & Salovey,
1993). A positive relationship with MBTI
iNtuition is strong support for the validity of this
dimension.
Another recent study also provides support for
the validity of the polarities assessed by the
MBTI. Furnham, Moutafi, and Crump (2003)
tested 900 adults with two instruments: the
MBTI and the Revised NEO-Personality
Inventory (NEO-PI-R, Costa & McCrae, 1992).
The NEO-PI-R is a well validated measure of
personality that evaluates five factors of
personality known as the “big five.” These
factors are Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness
(to experience), Agreeableness, and
Conscientiousness. As predicted by the authors,
the MBTI dimensions revealed healthy and
appropriate correlations with corresponding
factors from the NEO-PI-R. Specifically, the
following averaged concurrent validity
correlations were found between the MBTI
dimensions and the NEO-PI-R scales: E-I
correlated .71 with Extraversion; S-N correlated
−.65 with Openness; T-F correlated −.35 with
Agreeableness; and, J-P correlated .46 with
Conscientiousness. The negative correlations
indicate an inverse relationship, that is, those
categorized as S (Sensing) on the MBTI
obtained low scores on Openness, whereas those
categorized as N (iNtuition) obtained high
scores on Openness. In like manner a T or
Thinking type tended to obtain low scores on
Agreeableness whereas an F or Feeling type
tended to obtain high scores. All of these
correlations are consistent with theoretical
understandings of the MBTI and hence buttress
the validity of the instrument.
As mentioned, recent versions of the MBTI
yield additional information beyond the four-
letter typological classification. For example,
the 144-item form Q, available on-line, provides
a highly detailed and sophisticated summary
report that partitions each of the four polarities
into five facet scores. Hence the report includes
a total of 20 facet scores in addition to the four-
letter code. For example, the Thinking-Feeling
dimension includes bipolar facets such as
Logical-Empathetic, Reasonable-
Compassionate, and Tough-Tender. The
dimensions and facets of this version of the
MBTI are displayed in Table 9.1. The report
includes not only the typological classifications
(e.g., T or F) but also a rating for each bipolar
facet on an 11-point continuum. This kind of
nuanced dimensional information appeals to
many users.
TABLE 9.1 Dimensions and Facets of the
MBTI, Form Q
Extraversion (E) (I) Introversion
Initiating Receiving
Expressive Contained
Gregarious Intimate
Active Reflective
Enthusiastic Quiet
Sensing (S) (N) Intuition
Concrete Abstract
Realistic Imaginative
Practical Conceptual
Experiential Theoretical
Traditional Original
One concern about the MBTI is that the
increasing cost of administering the instrument
—in the range of $10 to $30 per individual—
provides a disincentive for outside researchers
who want to conduct reliability or validity
studies. This is an issue not only for the MBTI
but also for the most widely used contemporary
tests. Understandably, test publishers want to
profit from their massive and expensive efforts
at test development. But the downside is that
scholarly researchers need substantial funding if
they desire to administer newer versions of the
Thinking (T) (F) Feeling
Logical Empathetic
Reasonable Compassionate
Questioning Accommodating
Critical Accepting
Tough Tender
Judging (J) (P) Perceiving
Systematic Casual
Planful Open-Ended
Early Starting Pressure-Prompted
Scheduled Spontaneous
Methodical Emergent
MBTI to large samples of examinees. Partly in
reaction to the paucity of independent research
on newer versions of this test, reviewers
continue to suggest caution in its use, especially
when making simplistic inferences from the
four-letter type formulas (Pittenger, 2005).
9.3 CALIFORNIA
PSYCHOLOGICAL
INVENTORY (CPI)
Originally published in 1957, the CPI is a true–
false test designed expressly to measure the
dimensions of normal personality (Gough &
Bradley, 1996; McAllister, 1988). The
instrument is available in two forms, the
CPI-434 (Gough, 1995) and the CPI-260
(www.skillsone.com), which is available only
online. The component scales and the
interpretive strategies are nearly identical for the
two versions, which differ mainly in the number
of items—434 versus 260. Psychometric
properties of both versions are similar and
strong. Because of its ease of administration and
http://www.skillsone.com/
the immediacy with which the practitioner
receives an extensive computer-generated
report, the CPI-260 rapidly is gaining favor
among psychological practitioners.
The CPI-260 is scored for 20 folk measures of
personality, 7 work-related scales, and 3 broad
vectors. The purpose of the test is to provide a
clear picture of the examinee by using
descriptors based on the ordinary language of
everyday life (Gough & Bradley, 1996). Three
of the basic personality scales also provide
information on test-taking attitudes and
therefore function as validity scales. These
scales are Good Impression (Gi), which assesses
the extent to which the individual presents a
favorable image to others; Communality (Cm),
which measures unusual responses that might
arise from carelessness or faking bad; and Well-
being (Wb), which gauges the portrayal of
serious emotional problems.
TABLE 9.2 Brief Description of Standard
and Work-Related CPI-260 Scales
Standard
Scales
Common Interpretation of High
Score
D
o
Dominance dominant, persistent, good
leadership ability
C
s
Capacity for
Status
personal qualities that underlie
and lead to status
S
y
Sociability outgoing, sociable, participative
temperament
S
p
Social
Presence
poise, spontaneity, and self-
confidence in social situations
S
a
Self-
acceptance
self-acceptance and sense of
personal worth
I
n
Independen
ce
high sense of personal
independence, not easily
influenced
E
m
Empathy good capacity to empathize with
other persons
R
e
Responsibili
ty
conscientious, responsible, and
dependable
S
o
Social
Conformity
strong social maturity and high
integrity
S
c
Self-control good self-control, freedom from
impulsivity and self-
centeredness
G
i
Good
Impression
concerned about creating a good
impression
C
m
Communalit
y
valid and thoughtful response
pattern
W
b
Sense of
Well-being
not worrying or complaining,
free from self doubt
T
o
Tolerance permissive, accepting, and
nonjudgmental social beliefs
A
c
Achieveme
nt via
Conformanc
e
achieves well in settings where
conformance is necessary
A
i
Achieveme
nt via
Independen
ce
achieves well in settings where
independence is necessary
C
f
Conceptual
fluency
high degree of personal and
intellectual efficiency
Is Insightfulne
ss
interested in and responsive to
the inner needs, motives, and
experiences of others
F
x
Flexibility flexible and adaptable in thought
and social behavior
Source: Based on Gough, H. G. and Bradley, P. (1996).
CPI manual (3rd ed.). Mountain View, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press. Also, Megargee, E. (1972). The
S
n
Sensitivity sensitive to others’ feelings,
personally vulnerable
Work-
Related
Scales
Common Interpretation of High
Score
M
p
Managerial
Potential
good judgment, effective at
dealing with people
W
o
Work
Orientation
strong work ethic, rarely
complains about work
C
t
Creative
Temperame
nt
creative thinker who prefers
what is new or different
L
p
Leadership strong leadership skills, deals
well with stress
A
m
i
Amicability collegial and cooperative, a
good team player
L
e
o
Law
Enforcemen
t
Orientation
practical, well suited to work in
law enforcement
California Psychological Inventory handbook. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass; and McAllister, L. (1988). A
practical guide to CPI interpretation. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press.
The 20 folk measures and 7 work-related scales
are listed and briefly described in Table 9.2.
These scales are reported as T-scores normed to
a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 in
the general population. The test developers used
an empirical methodology of criterion-keying to
develop the majority of the scales. Specifically,
extreme groups of participants (mainly college
students) were formed on such scale-relevant
criteria as school grades, sociability, and
participation in curricular activities. Item-
endorsement frequencies were then contrasted
to ferret out the best statements for each scale.
For example, the Sociability (Sy) scale was
constructed by contrasting item-endorsement
rates for persons reporting a large number of
social activities versus those reporting few or no
social activities. In constructing four of the folk
scales, the authors used a rational basis backed
up by indices of internal consistency.
Reflecting the care with which the scales were
constructed, reliability data for the CPI are
respectable. Most alpha coefficients are in the
.70s and .80s, with a median value of .76. The
test–retest reliability coefficients tend to be
somewhat lower, with a median retest
correlation of .68. The authors provide a wealth
of normative data, including average test scores
for 52 samples of males and 42 samples of
females, subdivided by education, occupation,
college major, gender, and other variables. The
basic normative sample consists of 3,000 males
and 3,000 females of varying age, social class,
and geographic region (Gough & Bradley,
1996).
In addition to the wealth of information
provided by the individual scale scores, the CPI
also is scored on three broad dimensions or
vectors derived from decades of factor-analytic
studies with the instrument. The three vectors
include two basic orientations and a third theme
reflecting ego integration. The first basic
orientation called vector 1 or v.1 has two
polarities: toward people or toward one’s inner
life. This vector is similar to the extraversion–
introversion dimension found in nearly every
personality theory ever proposed. The second
basic orientation or v.2 also has two polarities:
rule-favoring or rule-questioning. This vector
reflects a conventional–unconventional
dimension also found in many studies. These
first two bipolar orientations, v.1 and v.2,
provide a 2 × 2 typology of four lifestyles
termed the Implementer, Supporter, Innovator,
and Visualizer lifestyles, described below. The
third vector or v.3 assesses a 7-point continuum
variously referred to as self-realization,
psychological competence, or ego integration.
In the client feedback report provided by the
publisher, v.3 is referred to as Level of
Satisfaction and scored 1 (low) to 7 (high). This
vector acts as a moderator for each of the
lifestyles, with high scores on v.3 leading to a
positive expression and low scores leading to a
negative expression.
Results from several correlational studies
confirm distinctive psychological portraits for
the four lifestyles mentioned above (Gough &
Bradley, 1996). Briefly, the four life styles are
as follows:
• Implementers (extroverted and rule-
favoring) tend to do well in managerial and
leadership roles.
• Supporters (introverted and rule-favoring)
function well in supportive or ancillary
positions.
• Innovators (extroverted and rule-
questioning) are adept at creating change.
• Visualizers (introverted and rule-
questioning) work best alone in fields such
as art or literature.
The CPI Manual provides a wealth of
information about each lifestyle, including
adjective correlates obtained from spouses,
peers, and professional evaluators. From these
empirical sources, a clear portrait of each
lifestyle emerges. For example, the summary
statement for Innovators is as follows:
Gammas attend to and seek the monetary,
prestige, and other rewards offered by
society, but are often at odds with the
culture concerning the criteria by which
these rewards are apportioned. Their values
are personal and individual, not traditional
or conventional. Gammas [Innovators] are
the doubters, the skeptics, those who see
and resist the arbitrary and unjustified
features of the status quo. At their best,
they are innovative and insightful creators
of new ideas, new products, and new social
forms. At their worst, they are rebellious,
intolerant, self-indulgent, and disruptive;
and at low levels on the v.3 scale, they
often behave in wayward, rule-violating,
and narcissistic ways. (Gough & Bradley,
1996, p. 50)
The reader will notice that the third vector, v.3,
moderates the expression of the
Implementer lifestyle, for better or for
worse. When v.3 is high, the Implementer
is innovative and insightful. When v.3 is
low, the Implementer is wayward and
narcissistic. A similar pattern holds true for
the other three lifestyles—each can have a
positive or negative expression, depending
on the level of personal integration
reflected on the v.3 scale.
The CPI is heir to a long history of empirical
research that substantiates a number of real-
world correlates for distinctive test profiles. Due
to space limitations, we can only list several
prominent areas in which the value of the test
has been empirically confirmed. The CPI is
useful for helping predict the following:
• Psychological and physical health
• High school and college achievement
• Effectiveness of student-teachers
• Effectiveness of police and military
personnel
• Leadership and management success
The CPI is particularly effective at identifying
adolescents or adults who follow a delinquent or
criminal lifestyle. For example, Gough and
Bradley (1992) studied a sample of 672
delinquent or criminal men and women,
contrasting their CPI scale scores with a large
sample of controls. Of the 27 scales evaluated,
they found significant mean differences on 25
for men and 26 for women. The most
discriminating scale was Social Conformity
(So), which revealed healthy point-biserial
correlations of .54 for men and .58 for women.
They also found that low scores on v.3 (a
measure of ego integration) were associated
with greater incidence of delinquency. The
reader can find further details on the real-world
empirical correlates of CPI profiles in Groth-
Marnat (2003) and Hargrave and Hiatt (1989).
9.4 NEO PERSONALITY
INVENTORY-REVISED (NEO
PI-R)
The NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO
PIR) embodies decades of factor-analytic
research with clinical and normal adult
populations (Costa & McCrae, 1992). The test is
based upon the five-factor model of personality
described in the previous chapter. It is available
in two parallel forms consisting of 240 items
rated on a five-point dimension. An additional
three items are used to check validity. A shorter
version, the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-
FFI) is also available (Costa & McCrae, 1989).
We limit our discussion to the NEO PI-R. Form
S is for self-reports whereas Form R is for
outside observers (e.g., the spouse of a …
References for articles attached
Meyer, G. J., & Kurtz, J. E. (2006). Advancing Personality Assessment
Terminology: Time to Retire “Objective” and “Projective” As Personality Test
Descriptors. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(3), 223–225. https://doi-
org.proxy-library.ashford.edu/10.1207/s15327752jpa8703_01
Stedman, J. M., Essery, J., & McGeary, C. A. (2018). Projective Personality
Assessment: Evidence for a Decline in Training Emphasis. SIS Journal of
Projective Psychology & Mental Health, 25(1), 54–59.
Santacreu, J., Rubio, V. J., & Hernández, J.,M. (2006). The objective
assessment of personality: Cattellss T-data revisited and more1. Psychology
Science, 48(1), 53-68. Retrieved from https://www-proquest-com.proxy-
library.ashford.edu/scholarly-journals/objective-assessment-personality-
cattellss-t-data/docview/212203756/se-2?accountid=32521
Gregory, R. J. (2014). Psychological testing: History, principles, and
applications (7th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.Chapter 8: Origins of Personality
Testing. Chapter 9: Assessment of Normality and Human Strengths
https://doi-org.proxy-library.ashford.edu/10.1207/s15327752jpa8703_01
https://doi-org.proxy-library.ashford.edu/10.1207/s15327752jpa8703_01
https://www-proquest-com.proxy-library.ashford.edu/scholarly-journals/objective-assessment-personality-cattellss-t-data/docview/212203756/se-2?accountid=32521
https://www-proquest-com.proxy-library.ashford.edu/scholarly-journals/objective-assessment-personality-cattellss-t-data/docview/212203756/se-2?accountid=32521
https://www-proquest-com.proxy-library.ashford.edu/scholarly-journals/objective-assessment-personality-cattellss-t-data/docview/212203756/se-2?accountid=32521
https://ashford.instructure.com/courses/91303/external_tools/retrieve?display=borderless&url=https\%3A\%2F\%2Fcontent.ashford.edu\%2Flti\%3Fbookcode\%3DGregory.8055.17.1
https://ashford.instructure.com/courses/91303/external_tools/retrieve?display=borderless&url=https\%3A\%2F\%2Fcontent.ashford.edu\%2Flti\%3Fbookcode\%3DGregory.8055.17.1
GUIDELINES EDITORIAL
MEYER AND KURTZADVANCING ASSESSMENT TERMINOLOGY
Advancing Personality Assessment Terminology:
Time to Retire “Objective” and “Projective”
As Personality Test Descriptors
Gregory J. Meyer
Department of Psychology
University of Toledo
John E. Kurtz
Department of Psychology
Villanova University
For decades psychologists have classified personality tests
dichotomously as objective or projective. These terms appear
in scientific articles and textbooks and have become so en-
trenched that it is common to see separate courses in graduate
clinical programs using these labels in course titles (e.g.,
“Objective Assessment,” “Projectives”). In the interest of ad-
vancing the science of personality assessment, we believe it
is time to end this historical practice and retire these terms
from our formal lexicon and general discourse describing the
methods of personality assessment.
For personality tests, the term objective typically refers to
instruments in which the stimulus is an adjective, proposi-
tion, or question that is presented to a person who is required
to indicate how accurately it describes his or her personality
using a limited set of externally provided response options
(true vs. false, yes vs. no, Likert scale, etc.). What is objective
about such a procedure is that the psychologist administering
the test does not need to rely on judgment to classify or inter-
pret the test-taker’s response; the intended response is clearly
indicated and scored according to a pre-existing key. As a re-
sult, however, the necessity for judgment is passed on to the
test taker. She must interpret the question, consider her per-
sonal characteristics, evaluate herself relative to others as
best she can, decide the extent to which the characteristic fits
her personality, and then choose whether to honestly convey
this information in her response.
On the other hand, the term projective typically refers to
instruments in which the stimulus is a task or activity that is
presented to a person who is required to generate a response
with minimal external guidance or constraints imposed on
the nature of that response. What is projective in a test like
this is the requirement to generate a response in the face of
ambiguity; in so doing, the person projects or puts forward
elements of her personal characteristics.
Unfortunately, the terms objective and projective carry
multiple, often unclear, meanings, including some connota-
tions that are very misleading when applied to personality as-
sessment instruments and methods. For instance, the term
objective implies accuracy and precision that is impervious
to biasing influences. These are desirable and positive con-
notations. One problem is that these positive connotations
are not fully warranted for the inventories to which they typi-
cally refer. Scoring errors are certainly one potential concern
(e.g., Allard & Faust, 2000). More substantively, however, if
the kind of self-report scales that are classified as objective
actually were “objective” in a meaningful sense of that word,
then there would not be such a huge literature examining the
various response styles and biases that affect scores derived
from these instruments. In fact, the literature addressing the
topic of response styles, malingering, and test bias in these
measures appears larger than the literature on any other fo-
cused issue concerning their validity or application. Beyond
bias and frank distortion, Meehl (1945) pointed out more
than half a century ago that the processes influencing a test-
taker’s response include ambiguity inherent in the test items,
limitations in self-knowledge or self-perception, personal
dynamics, and even projections. Another serious issue that
results from applying the term objective to certain personal-
ity instruments is that those so labeled will tend to be viewed
positively simply by virtue of the term’s positive connota-
tions. Tests that are not so categorized will tend to be viewed
less positively, regardless of psychometric data, because they
are, after all, not objective. Accordingly, an unintended con-
sequence of this terminology is that it may encourage or per-
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT, 87(3), 223–225
Copyright © 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
petuate prejudices regarding the many alternative methods of
assessment that do not carry the objective label.
At the same time, the connotations of the term projective
also do not always apply when considering the instruments
typically classified as projective. For instance, responses to
the Rorschach inkblots often have more to do with stimulus
classification and problem solving styles than to projection
in a classical Freudian sense of the term, where undesirable
personal feelings or impulses are seen as residing outside the
self (see Exner, 1989). Similar difficulties emerge when con-
sidering the expanded definition of the term projective as
Frank (1939) first defined it in reference to types of personal-
ity tests. Frank considered a projective test one that would
induce the individual to reveal his way of organizing experi-
ence by giving him a field (objects, materials, experiences)
with relatively little structure and cultural patterning so that
the personality can project upon that plastic field his way of
seeing life, his meanings, significances, patterns, and espe-
cially his feelings. Thus we elicit a projection of the individ-
ual personality’s private world because he has to organize the
field, interpret the material and react affectively to it. … The
important and determining process is the subject’s personal-
ity which operates upon the stimulus-situation as if it had a
wholly private significance for him alone or an entirely plas-
tic character which made it yield to the subject’s control.
(italics in the original; pp. 402–403)
This conceptualization of a projective test implies that
stimulus features or task requirements are essentially imma-
terial; personality characteristics will shine through with
force and clarity regardless of the medium. Although desir-
able, this view is clearly incorrect. For instance, it is well
documented that the largest source of variability in Ror-
schach scores is the number and complexity of responses
given (e.g., Meyer, 1993, 1997). The personality characteris-
tics associated with this style of responding are interpretively
quite important in their own right. However, the presence of
this response complexity confounds efforts to interpret the
test scores that psychologists are most interested in interpret-
ing (e.g., Exner, 2003).1 The situation is similar with the-
matic storytelling techniques, in which the number of words
given and the specific stimulus pictures selected for use exert
a powerful influence on the final scores obtained (e.g.,
Blankenship et al., 2006; Hibbard et al., 1994; Pang &
Schultheiss, 2005).
Thus, the old and familiar terminology of objective and pro-
jective personality tests has misleading connotations that will
not serve the field well as we seek to have a more differentiated
understanding of assessment methods. A relevant question
then becomes: What is better alternative terminology?
It is fairly easy to identify reasonable alternatives to sup-
plant the term objective. Almost exclusively, this term has
been applied to structured questionnaires that are completed
by the target person him or herself. Consequently, a reason-
able alternative is to refer to these tests as “self-report inven-
tories” or “patient-rated questionnaires.” Moreover, to
advance the science of assessment, it is equally important to
differentiate self-report inventories from inventories com-
pleted by knowledgeable informants. Given that sources of
information in personality assessment are far from inter-
changeable (e.g., Achenbach, Krukowski, Dumenci, &
Ivanova, 2005; Achenbach, McConaughy, & Howell, 1987;
Costa & McCrae, 1992; De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005;
Kraemer, Measelle, Ablow, Essex, Boyce, & Kupfer, 2003;
Meyer, 2002; Meyer et al., 2001), it would be optimal to fur-
ther differentiate all questionnaire methods by specifying the
type of informant providing judgments. Thus, peer ratings
would be labeled as such and differentiated from spouse-
report scales, parent-rated questionnaires, and so forth.
It is not as easy to identify a single term or phrase that could
supplant the term projective. In fact, when discussing this is-
sue with colleagues, disagreements about a suitable substitute
appear to be one of the greatest obstacles to change. No single
term seems fully adequate. The instruments that are typically
subsumed under the projective label include the Rorschach
(1921/1942) and other inkblot tests (e.g., Holtzman, Thorpe,
Swartz, & Herron, 1961), Murray’s (1943) Thematic
Apperception Test and the subsequently developed Picture
Story Exercise stimuli (e.g., Smith, 1992), sentence comple-
tion measures, and various figure drawing tasks (e.g., Naglieri
& Pfeiffer, 1992). The wide differences among these tasks
makes it challenging to find a suitable alternative term that ac-
commodates all of their diverse features. Some possibilities
include “performance tasks,” “behavioral tasks,” “construc-
tive methods,” “free response measures,” “expressive person-
ality tests,” “implicit methods,” or even “attributive tests.” It is
unlikely that any one of these labels would satisfy all experts.
However, it is the very difficulty of finding a suitable alterna-
tive that speaks to the inadvisability of using a global term to
characterize the essence of all these measures. In turn, this
highlights the need to drop the term projective from the assess-
ment method lexicon.
One of the initial steps to advance the scientific understand-
ing of any phenomenon is to name and classify its components
in a meaningful way. The unsuitable and primitive nature of
the term projective is revealed when trying to arrive at an um-
brella label to characterize tasks as diverse as drawing one’s
family, telling stories in response to pictures, and stating what
an inkblot looks like. Applying a global and undifferentiated
term to such a diverse array of assessment tasks seems akin to
physicians classifying medical tests as either “visual tests” or
“nonvisual tests,” with the visual category including tasks
ranging from observing reflexes to endoscopy to MRI, and the
nonvisual category including tasks ranging from palpation
224 MEYER AND KURTZ
1The confounding influence of this so called “first factor” vari-
ance is pervasive with other instruments as well. An excellent dis-
cussion of the problem and of a sophisticated effort to mitigate its in-
fluence on the MMPI–2 can be found in the recently published
Special Issue of the Journal of Personality Assessment (Meyer,
2006) dealing with the MMPI–2 Restructured Clinical Scales
(Tellegen et al., 2003).
methods (e.g., abdominal tenderness) to olfactory methods
(e.g., odors indicative of infection) to auditory methods (e.g.,
detecting wheezes with a stethoscope).
Just as it would be regressive to apply such a simplistic cate-
gorization to medical tests, the field of personality assessment
will not advance by relying on crude terminology to globally
characterize all the tasks that are not self-report questionnaires
or informant rating scales. Thus, if one of the substitute terms
noted above does not seem suitable to replace projective, it
would be most optimal for clinicians, researchers, and teach-
ers to simply refer to assessment tasks by their specific name,
for example, the Rorschach Inkblot Method, Holtzman Ink-
blot Task, Murray’s TAT, Loevinger’s SCT. The Journal of
Personality Assessment will facilitate the transition to more
adequately differentiated assessment terminology by asking
authors to avoid referring to categories of personality tests as
objective or projective. We hope other assessment journals
will join this effort and adopt a similar position.
This editorial guideline is not meant to imply that the words
objective and projective cannot be used in the context of refer-
ring to specific data from personality instruments. It is cer-
tainly true that all personality tests can provide more or less
objective data. It is also the case that instruments like the Ror-
schach or TAT can capture projected personality characteris-
tics, whether defined narrowly as by Freud or more broadly as
by Frank, and this can also occur when patients complete self-
report inventories (Meehl, 1945). There is no problem if au-
thors carefully and deliberately choose these terms to further
scientific communication (e.g., when one is describing as-
pects of inkblot responses that are truly believed to indicate
projected dynamics). Rather, our objection is with the reflex-
ive use of historically ingrained terms that poorly describe the
complex and distinctive methods used to assess personality.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Our thoughts on this topic have benefited from helpful and
insightful input from many people. Those who commented
on this document included Robert Bornstein, Anita Boss,
Virginia Brabender, Philip Caracena, Robert Erard, Barton
Evans, Leonard Handler, Radhika Krishnamurthy, Robert
McGrath, Joni Mihura, David Nichols, Bruce Smith, Donald
Viglione, Irving Weiner, and Jed Yalof.
REFERENCES
Achenbach, T. M., Krukowski, R. A., Dumenci, L., & Ivanova, M. Y. (2005).
Assessment of adult psychopathology: Meta-analyses and implications of
cross-informant correlations. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 361–382.
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adoles-
cent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant
correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.
Allard, G., & Faust, D. (2000). Errors in scoring objective personality tests.
Assessment, 7, 119–129.
Blankenship, V., Vega, C. M., Ramos, E., Romero, K., Warren, K., Keenan,
K., et al. (2006). Using the multifaceted Rasch model to improve the
TAT/PSE measure of need for achievement. Journal of Personality As-
sessment, 86, 100–114.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory
(NEO–PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI) professional
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies in the
assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical
framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulle-
tin, 131, 483–509.
Exner, J. E. (1989). Searching for projection in the Rorschach. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 53, 520–536.
Exner, J. E. (2003). The Rorschach: A Comprehensive System (4th ed.). New
York: Wiley.
Frank, L. K. (1939). Projective methods for the study of personality. Journal
of Psychology, 8, 389–413.
Hibbard, S., Farmer, L., Wells, C., Difillipo, E., Barry, W., Korman, R., &
Sloan, P. (1994). Validation of Cramer’s Defense Mechanism Manual for
the TAT. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 197–210.
Holtzman, W. H., Thorpe, J. S., Swartz, J. D., & Herron, E. W. (1961). Ink-
blot perception and personality. Austin: University of Texas.
Kraemer, H. C., Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., &
Kupfer, D. J. (2003). A new approach to integrating data from multiple infor-
mants in psychiatric assessment and research: Mixing and matching contexts
and perspectives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1566–1577.
Meehl, P. E. (1945). The dynamics of “structured” personality tests. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 1, 296–303
Meyer, G. J. (1993). The impact of response frequency on the Rorschach
constellation indices and on their validity with diagnostic and MMPI–2
criteria. Journal of Personality Assessment, 60, 153–180.
Meyer, G. J. (1997). On the integration of personality assessment methods: The
Rorschach and MMPI. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 297–330.
Meyer, G. J. (2002). Implications of information-gathering methods for a re-
fined taxonomy of psychopathology. In L. E. Beutler & M. Malik (Eds.),
Rethinking the DSM: Psychological perspectives (pp. 69–105). Washing-
ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Meyer, G. J. (Ed.). (2006). The MMPI–2 Restructured Clinical Scales [Spe-
cial Issue]. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(2).
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R.,
et al. (2001). Psychological testing and psychological assessment: A re-
view of evidence and issues. American Psychologist, 56, 128–165.
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test manual. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Naglieri, J. A., & Pfeiffer, S. I. (1992). Validity of the Draw A Person:
Screening Procedure For Emotional Disturbance with a socially-
emotionally disturbed sample. Psychological Assessment, 4, 156–159.
Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U.S. col-
lege students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and
cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 280–294.
Rorschach, H. (1942). Psychodiagnostics (5th ed.). Berne, Switzerland:
Verlag Hans Huber. (Original work published 1921)
Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of the-
matic content analysis. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tellegen, A., Ben-Porath, Y. S., McNulty, J. L., Arbisi, P. A., Graham, J. R.,
& Kaemmer, B. (2003). The MMPI-2 Restructured Clinical Scales: De-
velopment, validation, and interpretation. Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press.
Gregory J. Meyer
Department of Psychology
Mail Stop 948
University of Toledo
2801 Bancroft Street
Toledo, OH 43606
Email: [email protected]
ADVANCING ASSESSMENT TERMINOLOGY 225
SIS J. Proj. Psy. &Ment. Health (2018) 25: 54-59
James M. Stedman, PhD, ABPP, Joshua Essery, PhD, ABPP and Cindy A. McGeary, PhD, ABPP, The University of
Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, Texas (Corresponding Author: James M. Stedman, PhD ABPP, Department
of Psychiatry, University of Texas Health Science Center, 7703 Floyd Curl Drive, San Antonio, Texas 78229 – 7792,
Email: [email protected], Phone: 210-567-5488, Fax: 210-567-5677).
Keywords: Projective Tests, Rorschach, Thematic Apperception Test, Internship Training, Graduate Clinical Training
Projective Personality Assessment: Evidence for a Decline in Training Emphasis
James M. Stedman, Joshua Essery and Cindy A. McGeary
The following paper provides an overview and discussion of past and present trends in training related to
projective personality assessment. The authors summarize the historical use of projective testing (particularly the
Rorschach) and emerging research examining the use of projectives in both graduate and internship training
programs. The current state of the research suggests a decrease in emphasis in training with projective
techniques across both graduate and internship training programs. This paper further discusses the University of
Texas Health San Antonio clinical psychology internship program as one that continues to value and train interns
in projective assessment. The authors end this paper with a brief discussion of the reasons for the decline i n
projective assessment across graduate schools and internship sites.
Projective personality assessment, in its
heyday, centered on a cluster of instruments:
the Rorschach Inkblot Test, the Thematic
Apperception Test, various figure drawing
tests, and various versions of the Sentence
Completion Test, and post WWII clinical
psychology graduate training programs
devoted considerable training time to teaching
personality assessment with these
instruments (see Weiner & Greene. 2008).
With the advent of formal internship programs,
training with these “projective” instruments
extended until completion of the doctoral
program.
The projective instruments were intertwined
with psychoanalytic theory and were thought
of as revealing the underlying dynamics, such
as conflicts, drives, motives and such.
However, the dominant test, the Rorschach,
was also seen as having other functions, such
as measuring a person‟s reality testing (Beck,
1944) and offering ratios to organize other
more psychometric-like features (Klopfer&
Davidson. 1962). While the TAT, drawings,
and sentence completion instruments
remained tied to their projective roots, the
Rorschach underwent a major transformation
„with the advent of the Exner scoring system
(1969) which deemphasized psychoanalytic
interpretation and shifted toward a cognitive
analysis emphasizing information processing,
mediation, and ideation. These data are then
organized into a structural summary and
interpreted in relation to research findings
related to personality characteristics. Exner‟s
scoring method was designed to correct
criticisms of several scoring methods in use at
the time and rather quickly became the
method most emphasized in the majority of
graduate and internship programs. This
tradition in Rorschach assessment has now
been supplemented with the Rorschach
Performance Assessment System (Meyer et
al. 2011). Bram and Yalof (2018) offer a
depth analysis of the pros and cons for CS
versus R-PAS.
This brief (and hopefully mostly accurate)
history sets the scene for discussion and
review regarding current patterns of projective
assessment training at both the graduate
school and internship levels.
Graduate-level Assessment Training:
Piotrowski (2015) has provided a very
comprehensive review of the status of
graduate school training with the traditional
projective techniques. This review highlighted
12 surveys, conducted between 1995 and
2014 on instruction with projective
instruments. This analysis identified a
Training in Traditional Personality Assessment: 55
precipitous decline in training emphasis with
projective assessment. An earlier study by
Hilsenroth and Handler (1995), involving 166
graduate students, found that 50\% completed
two courses with the Rorschach and 32\%
administered 5 to 9 protocols. A later study
by Belter and Piotrowski (2001) surveyed 82
APA-approved clinical doctoral programs and
found that 60\% required projective training,
primarily with the Rorschach and TAT.
Nevertheless, 50 \% these training directors
confirmed a decrease in instruction with
projective instruments in the previous five
years. Childs and Ende (2002) found similar
results in a survey of 84 APA- approved
clinical program and reported that 87\% of
respondents offered a least one course in
projective testing, with training emphasizing
the Rorschach and TAT.
Of special note is a recent study by Ready
and Veague (2014) which investigated
training in assessment across three training
models (Clinical-Science, Scientist-
Practitioner, and Practitioner-Scholar). They
found that projective tests were rated
significantly below “evidence-based” tests,
that only Scholar-Practitioner programs
tended to offer projective instruction, and that
younger faculty was not interested in teaching
projective techniques. This study is perhaps
the most comprehensive attempt to gather
needed information about current projective
personality assessment teaching trends but
was hampered by low response rates (32\%).
Nevertheless, it is the most comprehensive
study to date and clearly demonstrated a
decline in teaching traditional projective
instruments.
Another rich source of data regarding current
assessment training during graduate school is
the APPIC Application for Psychology
Internship (AAPI). Every year all applicants
for internships fill in extensive data regarding
their assessment training experience during
graduate school, including course work,
practica, and write-up/interpretation
requirements. To date, two studies have
mined this data (Stedman, Hatch, &
Schoenfeld, 2001b; Stedman, Hatch, &
Schoenfeld, 2002). The earlier study used
AAPI forms collected from multiple internship
sites for a total of 238 clinical and 96
counseling students, most of whom were from
different programs. The dependent variable
was “number of test reports written” without
reference to particular test. Results indicated
that clinical students claimed a median of 18
reports and counseling students reported a
median of 11. The second study of the same
sample is of more direct relevance because it
broke down “number of test reports written” by
test. Findings were the following for clinical
students and are reported as medians:
Rorschach 4.3, TAT 1.5, sentences 2.0, and
drawings 1.0. For counseling students, results
were Rorschach 2.0, TAT 2.0, sentences 2.0,
and drawings 1.3.
In summary, Piotrowski‟s review and the
studies cited above offer some quantitative
analysis of the status of graduate school
instruction with the traditional projective
techniques. Those studies and others
(Piotrowski & Zalewski. 1993; Watkins et al.
1991) appear to show that graduate school
teaching of the traditional projective tests
declined in the 1990s, declined further during
the later 2000s, and declined perhaps
precipitously by 2014. However, it should be
noted that none of these investigations
captured the data needed to make a definitive
statement due primarily to the difficulty related
to obtaining sufficient samples.
Our suggestion is that systematic data
collection from APPI forms would be the best
way to capture the samples needed to
ascertain current testing instruction patterns
during graduate school. This would require
multisite participation of graduate schools or
an arrangement with APPIC to tap into
samples of APPI forms or both.
Internship Training in Assessment:
There are many more studies of clinical
instruction during internship training than
during graduate school training. These
investigations include all aspects relevant to
clinical and counseling psychology, including
56: Stedman, Essery McGeary
testing, and tend to have better sampling of
the population, hence, increased confidence
regarding generalization. A review by
Stedman (1997) noted that three studies in
the 1980s (Durand, Blanchard, & Mindell,
1988; Piotrowski, 1984; Petzel& Berndt, 1980)
documented that internship directors valued
training with projective instruments but found
less support for projective training by the
“academic community”. Strong support for
training with the Rorschach and TAT was
demonstrated in a study by Stedman, Hatch,
and Schoenfeld (2000). They asked
internship directors to indicate the number of
integrated test write-ups, including the
Rorschach and TAT, they wanted from
applicants. Results varied across internship
settings but ranged from 1.0 (Counseling
Centers) to 20.0 (Private Psychiatric
Hospitals) for the Rorschach. Programs
wanted fewer TAT reports but again ranged
from 1.0 (Consortia) to 21.0 (Private
Psychiatric Hospitals). An additional study by
Stedman, Hatch and Schoenfeld (2001a)
asked internship directors to rate the
importance of pre-internship preparation in
test-based assessment and psychotherapy.
Although directors rated perpetration on
objective testing instruments higher (range
across types of sites 5.0 to 3.7 on a 5 point
scale), still they rated training in projective
assessment at significant levels (4.4 to 2.8,
with most sites rating above 3.0). Hence,
internships continued to value training with
projective instruments. Other studies of
internship sites, such as Piotrowski & Belter
(1999) and Clemence& Handler (2001), found
similar results.
There appears to be a gap in direct studies of
internship training with projective techniques
or methods between the early 2000s and very
recently. In fact, Piotrowski asked the first
author (Stedman) for an opinion about the
status of projective training during internship
in 2015, and I speculated that internship
training with projectives had indeed declined.
Piotrowski‟s question called for an empirical
answer and one is now available. Stedman,
McGeary, & Essery (in press) surveyed the
733 APPIC internship programs and obtained
a 46\% response rate (355 programs). These
investigators asked internship directors to
indicate the following: a) whether they
expected personality assessment training
during graduate school, b) whether they
expected training with specific tests (MMPI,
PAI, MCMI, Rorschach, Picture Story,
Sentence Completion, Drawings) and their
ratings of the importance of that training, and
c) their own patterns of training with these
instruments (the most important question for
our purposes). Results were reported for
three categories of internship programs:
Adults Only, Child Only, and Mixed Child and
Adult. Findings confirmed the speculation that
internship programs have severely cut training
with the traditional projective instruments.
Training with the Rorschach occurs in only
26\% of Adult Only, 32\% of Child Only, and
33\% (Adult rotations) and 35\% (Child
rotations) of Mixed programs. Training with
story techniques (TAT, Roberts, etc.) is as
follows: Adult Only 19\%, Child Only 39\%,
Mixed – Child 41\%, Adult 29\%. Training with
Sentence Completion instruments and
drawings are even lower, although, as seen
above, child programs tend higher.
These findings show definitively that rates of
training with projective methods have declined
to the graduate school levels. In this most
recent study, we also tried to ascertain
reasons for not providing instruction with
these instruments. The response rate for
these questions was rather low; however,
those responding cited a lack of trained staff,
“too labor intensive”, and questionable validity
of these types of tests as reasons for not
conducting training.
UTHSCSA: An Internship Program Continuing
to Teach Projective Assessment:
One example of an internship program
continuing to train and teach projective
measures is The University of Texas Health
Science Center at San Antonio (UTHSCSA)
which offers an APA accredited pre-doctoral
internship program through the Department of
Psychiatry. The internship operates from a
Training in Traditional Personality Assessment: 57
generalist training model whereby a number
of sites in the area offer rotations for interns
who also obtain training and learning
opportunities at UTHSCA. Whereas the
overall internship is generalist in model,
UTHSCSA offers three different tracks in
order to provide interns more in depth training
in a desired area of specialization. The three
tracts for the internship are 1) Forensic, 2)
Cognitive Behavioral, and 3) Psychodynamic.
UTHSCA Forensic and Psychodynamic
interns receive exposure to projective
measures to differing degrees. Cognitive
Behavioral interns do not receive training or
seminars on projective tests. For the Forensic
Track, interns have the opportunity to
administer as well as score projective tests
and write interpretive reports under
supervision. Forensic Track residents work
solely with an adult population. The
psychodynamic residents are provided
seminars on projective tests, case
conferences where projective materials are
discussed as well as opportunities to
administer, score and provide written
interpretation of projective tests under
supervision. Psychodynamic residents work
with mixed child and adult populations.
The most heavily utilized projective test
across tracks and populations is the
Rorschach. Supervisors have a diversity of
training backgrounds with the Rorschach (i.e.,
Beck, Klopfer, Exner, etc.) but the primary two
systems utilized are Exner and R-PAS. Other
projective tests used are story telling tests (i.e.
TAT and Roberts Apperception Test),
sentence completion tests and projective
drawings (i.e. House-Tree-Person, Family
Kinetic and School Kinetic, etc.). These tests
tend to be interpreted qualitatively utilizing a
combination of developmental and personality
theories to inform interpretation.
Interns for the psychodynamic track are
taught to administer the Rorschach in a
standardized manner utilizing both the Exner
and R-PAS systems. Interns are also provided
training in the Rapaport method, particularly
with younger children or for those who may
have a difficult time with the query phase of
the Exner or R-PAS approaches, usually due
to memory or frustration tolerance. Instruction
is provided on the limitations and criticisms of
the Rorschach in seminars and interns are
taught to administer the test only after
deciding if the Rorschach is an appropriate
measure for the referral questions being
considered. Interns are instructed to be aware
of context, age, cultural backgrounds, gender
and linguistic abilities when utilizing the
Rorschach and these variables are commonly
discussed during supervision, seminars and
case consultations. Rorschach interpretation
methods taught utilize protocol scores as
anchors for interpretation. Interpretive
methods emphasize the Authoritative,
Empirical and Conceptual approaches
outlined in chapter 2 of Weiner‟s (2003) text,
while what is referred to as the “Ouija Board”
approach to interpretation is cautioned.
Interns are taught a process of formulating
their interpretations utilizing what Weiner
(2003) outlines as structural, thematic, and
behavioral characteristics as well as
sequence analysis. Lastly, psychodynamic
interns are employed to utilize developmental
and personality theories to contextualize and
interpret findings obtained from the earlier
described process. This approach provides
direction in providing relevant feedback and
treatment recommendations to patients,
parents and referral sources.
Projective Training Circa 1963:
For those who have not been in the clinical
trenches 50+ years, it might be of some
interest to know what the “hayday” of
projective assessment was really like. That
task falls to the first author, Stedman. So, I
enrolled in the ”projectives” offering in the fall
1963, taught by Dr. John Napoli. His motto
was, “Napoli‟s the name; Rorschach‟s the
game.” So, we did the usual short review of
the TAT and proceeded to the Rorschach.
The personality assessment portion of a
typical report for Professor Napoli and later
during my internship went thusly:
58: Stedman, Essery McGeary
1. A paragraph devoted to the patient‟s
“reality contact” as determined by scoring
the protocol according to the Beck norms.
2. Further consideration of Klopfer ratios and
percentages, e.g., the ratio of Movement
responses to Sum C, the famous
Erlebenstype, indicating response to
external or internal stimuli; percent of F-
responses with F being Form scorings.
3. A lengthy paragraph or two regarding the
patient‟s psychodynamic issues as
determined first by the Rorschach and
backed up by the TAT, Sentence
Completion, and perhaps drawings. The
Rorschach was seen as getting at the
deepest level of psychodynamic conflict,
followed by the TAT. The Sentence
Completion and drawings were viewed, at
least in my programs, as more superficial
measures.
4. Rorschach analysis could occur in what
was called “sequence analysis”,
conducted on a card by card basis and, at
times, a response by response basis.
Certain cards had established “pull”. For
example, Card IV was the Father Card
and Card VII was the Mother Card. This
point was brought home to me during my
internship when, T, who we thought might
be having much conflict with mom, took
one look at Card VII, broke it across his
knee, and handed it to me. Analysis could
also be conducted by reference to the
book by Phillips and Smith (1953) who
had valuable psychodynamic
interpretations for individual types of
responses. We were told not to use this
text but with a wink.
5. After a lengthy session of puzzlement
over all these sources of data, you wrote it
all up as a comprehensive picture of the
patient.
Conclusions:
There is now little doubt that training with
traditional projective tests has declined at all
levels and with all instruments as noted by
Piotrowski (2015, 2017) in his reviews.
Piotrowski‟s conclusions also noted that
researchers and the academic community in
general began “hardened opposition” to the
projective test cluster in the late 1990s. In
fact, as early as 2000, an APA Division 12
Task force recommended that projective
techniques not be taught in clinical programs
(Lilienfeld et al. 2000) and several authors
recommended a” moratorium” for teaching
projective instruments (e.g., Wood et al.
2000). Piotrowski speculates that this
opposition, the effects of managed care, and
other factors (we suspect that the move away
from dynamic personality theory and toward
behavioral and cognitive-behavioral
approaches in clinical training is a strong
factor) have contributed to this noteworthy
decline. Nevertheless, at the internship level,
some adult programs and more child program
continue to use and teach at least the
Rorschach and picture story techniques.
Findings, based on hard data, are much less
definitive regarding current training at the
graduate school level. However, this training
has not disappeared. Those seeking exposure
to projective assessment can still find
programs teaching and using projective
assessment techniques.
References:
Beck, S. (1944).Rorschach‟s test. New York: Grune and
Stratton.
Belter, R. W ., & Piotrowski, C. (2001).Current status of
doctoral-level training in psychological testing.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 717-726.
Bram, A.D., & Yalof, J. (2018). Two contemporary
Rorschach systems: Views of two experienced
Rorschachers on the CS and R-PAS. Journal of
Projective Psychology & Mental Health, 25, this
issue.
Childs, R. A., & Ende, L. D. (2002) Assessment training in
clinical doctoral programs: W hat should we
teach? W hat do we teach? Journal of Personality
Assessment,78, 130-144.
Clemence, A. J., & Handler, L. (2001). Psychological
assessment on internship: A survey of Training
directors and their expectations for students.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 76, 18-47.
Durand, M. V., Blanchard, E. B., & Mindell, J. A. (1988).
Training in projective testing: Survey of clinical
training directors and internship directors.
Training in Traditional Personality Assessment: 59
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
19, 236-238.
Exner, J. E. (1969). The Rorschach Systems. New York:
Grune and Stratton.
Hilsenroth, M. J., & Handler, L. (1995).A survey of
graduate students‟ experiences, interests, and
attitudes about learning the Rorschach. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 64, 243-257.
Klopfer, B., & Davidson, H. H. (1962). The Rorschach
technique: An introductory manual. New York:
Harcourt, Brace and W orld.
Lilenfeld, S. O., W ood, J. M., & Garb, H. N. (2000).The
scientific status of projective techniques.
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 1,
27-66.
Meyer, G. J., Vigilone, D. J., Mihura, J. L., Erard, R. E., &
Erdberg, P. (2011). Rorschach performance
assessment system: Administration, coding,
interpretation, and technical manual. Toledo, OH:
Rorschach Performance Assessment System.
Petzel, T.P., & Berndt, D.J. (1980). APA internship
selection criteria: Relative importance of
academic and clinical preparation. Professional
Psychology, 11, 792-796.
Phillips,L., & Smith, J.G. (1953). Rorschach interpretation:
Advanced technique. New York: Grune and
Stratton.
Piotrowski, C. (1984). The status of projective techniques:
“W ishing won‟t make it go away.” Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 40, 1495-1502.
Piotrowski, C. (2015). Clinical instruction on projective
techniques in the USA: A review of academic
training settings 1995 – 2014. Journal of
Projective Psychology and Mental Health, 22(2),
83-92.
Piotrowski, C. (2017). The linchpin on the future of
projective techniques: The precarious statusof
personality assessment in the (overcrowded)
professional psychology curriculum. Journal of
Projective Psychology and Mental Health, 24, 71-
73.
Piotrowski, C., & Belter, R. W . (1999). Internship training
in psychological assessment: Has managed care
had an impact? Assessment, 6, 381-389.
Piotrowski, C., & Zalewski, C. (1993).Training in
psychodiagnostic testing in APA-approved PsyD
and PhD clinical psychology programs. Journal of
Personality Assessment,61(2), 394-405.
Ready R. E., & Veague, H. B. (2014). Training in
psychological assessment: Current practices in
clinical psychology programs. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 45, 278-282.
Stedman J. M., Hatch, J. P., & Schoenfeld (2002).Pre-
internship preparation of clinical and counseling
students in psychological testing, psychotherapy,
and supervision: Their readiness for medical
school and non-medical school internships.
Journal of Clinical Psychological in Medical
Schools, 9, 267-271.
Stedman, J. M. (1997). W hat we know about predoctoral
internship training: A review. Professional
Psychology: Research & Practice, 28, 475-485.
Stedman, J. M., Hatch, J. P., & Schoenfeld, L. S.
(2000).Preinternship preparation in psychological
testing and psychotherapy: W hat internship
directors say they want. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 31, 321-326.
Stedman, J. M., Hatch, J. P., & Schoenfeld, L. S.
(2001b).The current status of psychological
assessment training in graduate and professional
schools. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77,
398-407.
Stedman, J. M., McGeary, C. A., & Essery, J. (In Press).
Current patterns of training in personality
assessment during internship. Journal of Clinical
Psychology.
W atkins, C. E. (1991). W hat have surveys taught us
about the teaching and practice of psychological
assessment? Journal of Personality Assessment,
56, 380-383.
W einer, I.B. (2003). Principles of Rorschach
Interpretation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
W einer, I.B., & Greene, R. (Eds.).(2008). Handbook of
personality assessment. Hoboken, NJ: W iley &
Sons.
W ood, J. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., Garb, H. N., & Nezworski,
Mt T. (2000). Limitations of the Rorschach as a
diagnostic tool: A reply to Garfield (2000), Lerner
(2000), and W einer (2000). Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 56, 441-448.
Copyright of SIS Journal of Projective Psychology & Mental Health is the property of Dubay
Business Services and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holders express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The objective assessment of personality: Cattellss T-data revisited and more1
Santacreu, José;Rubio, Victor J;Hernández, José M
Psychology Science; 2006; 48, 1; ProQuest Central
pg. 53
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CATEGORIES
Economics
Nursing
Applied Sciences
Psychology
Science
Management
Computer Science
Human Resource Management
Accounting
Information Systems
English
Anatomy
Operations Management
Sociology
Literature
Education
Business & Finance
Marketing
Engineering
Statistics
Biology
Political Science
Reading
History
Financial markets
Philosophy
Mathematics
Law
Criminal
Architecture and Design
Government
Social Science
World history
Chemistry
Humanities
Business Finance
Writing
Programming
Telecommunications Engineering
Geography
Physics
Spanish
ach
e. Embedded Entrepreneurship
f. Three Social Entrepreneurship Models
g. Social-Founder Identity
h. Micros-enterprise Development
Outcomes
Subset 2. Indigenous Entrepreneurship Approaches (Outside of Canada)
a. Indigenous Australian Entrepreneurs Exami
Calculus
(people influence of
others) processes that you perceived occurs in this specific Institution Select one of the forms of stratification highlighted (focus on inter the intersectionalities
of these three) to reflect and analyze the potential ways these (
American history
Pharmacology
Ancient history
. Also
Numerical analysis
Environmental science
Electrical Engineering
Precalculus
Physiology
Civil Engineering
Electronic Engineering
ness Horizons
Algebra
Geology
Physical chemistry
nt
When considering both O
lassrooms
Civil
Probability
ions
Identify a specific consumer product that you or your family have used for quite some time. This might be a branded smartphone (if you have used several versions over the years)
or the court to consider in its deliberations. Locard’s exchange principle argues that during the commission of a crime
Chemical Engineering
Ecology
aragraphs (meaning 25 sentences or more). Your assignment may be more than 5 paragraphs but not less.
INSTRUCTIONS:
To access the FNU Online Library for journals and articles you can go the FNU library link here:
https://www.fnu.edu/library/
In order to
n that draws upon the theoretical reading to explain and contextualize the design choices. Be sure to directly quote or paraphrase the reading
ce to the vaccine. Your campaign must educate and inform the audience on the benefits but also create for safe and open dialogue. A key metric of your campaign will be the direct increase in numbers.
Key outcomes: The approach that you take must be clear
Mechanical Engineering
Organic chemistry
Geometry
nment
Topic
You will need to pick one topic for your project (5 pts)
Literature search
You will need to perform a literature search for your topic
Geophysics
you been involved with a company doing a redesign of business processes
Communication on Customer Relations. Discuss how two-way communication on social media channels impacts businesses both positively and negatively. Provide any personal examples from your experience
od pressure and hypertension via a community-wide intervention that targets the problem across the lifespan (i.e. includes all ages).
Develop a community-wide intervention to reduce elevated blood pressure and hypertension in the State of Alabama that in
in body of the report
Conclusions
References (8 References Minimum)
*** Words count = 2000 words.
*** In-Text Citations and References using Harvard style.
*** In Task section I’ve chose (Economic issues in overseas contracting)"
Electromagnetism
w or quality improvement; it was just all part of good nursing care. The goal for quality improvement is to monitor patient outcomes using statistics for comparison to standards of care for different diseases
e a 1 to 2 slide Microsoft PowerPoint presentation on the different models of case management. Include speaker notes... .....Describe three different models of case management.
visual representations of information. They can include numbers
SSAY
ame workbook for all 3 milestones. You do not need to download a new copy for Milestones 2 or 3. When you submit Milestone 3
pages):
Provide a description of an existing intervention in Canada
making the appropriate buying decisions in an ethical and professional manner.
Topic: Purchasing and Technology
You read about blockchain ledger technology. Now do some additional research out on the Internet and share your URL with the rest of the class
be aware of which features their competitors are opting to include so the product development teams can design similar or enhanced features to attract more of the market. The more unique
low (The Top Health Industry Trends to Watch in 2015) to assist you with this discussion.
https://youtu.be/fRym_jyuBc0
Next year the $2.8 trillion U.S. healthcare industry will finally begin to look and feel more like the rest of the business wo
evidence-based primary care curriculum. Throughout your nurse practitioner program
Vignette
Understanding Gender Fluidity
Providing Inclusive Quality Care
Affirming Clinical Encounters
Conclusion
References
Nurse Practitioner Knowledge
Mechanics
and word limit is unit as a guide only.
The assessment may be re-attempted on two further occasions (maximum three attempts in total). All assessments must be resubmitted 3 days within receiving your unsatisfactory grade. You must clearly indicate “Re-su
Trigonometry
Article writing
Other
5. June 29
After the components sending to the manufacturing house
1. In 1972 the Furman v. Georgia case resulted in a decision that would put action into motion. Furman was originally sentenced to death because of a murder he committed in Georgia but the court debated whether or not this was a violation of his 8th amend
One of the first conflicts that would need to be investigated would be whether the human service professional followed the responsibility to client ethical standard. While developing a relationship with client it is important to clarify that if danger or
Ethical behavior is a critical topic in the workplace because the impact of it can make or break a business
No matter which type of health care organization
With a direct sale
During the pandemic
Computers are being used to monitor the spread of outbreaks in different areas of the world and with this record
3. Furman v. Georgia is a U.S Supreme Court case that resolves around the Eighth Amendments ban on cruel and unsual punishment in death penalty cases. The Furman v. Georgia case was based on Furman being convicted of murder in Georgia. Furman was caught i
One major ethical conflict that may arise in my investigation is the Responsibility to Client in both Standard 3 and Standard 4 of the Ethical Standards for Human Service Professionals (2015). Making sure we do not disclose information without consent ev
4. Identify two examples of real world problems that you have observed in your personal
Summary & Evaluation: Reference & 188. Academic Search Ultimate
Ethics
We can mention at least one example of how the violation of ethical standards can be prevented. Many organizations promote ethical self-regulation by creating moral codes to help direct their business activities
*DDB is used for the first three years
For example
The inbound logistics for William Instrument refer to purchase components from various electronic firms. During the purchase process William need to consider the quality and price of the components. In this case
4. A U.S. Supreme Court case known as Furman v. Georgia (1972) is a landmark case that involved Eighth Amendment’s ban of unusual and cruel punishment in death penalty cases (Furman v. Georgia (1972)
With covid coming into place
In my opinion
with
Not necessarily all home buyers are the same! When you choose to work with we buy ugly houses Baltimore & nationwide USA
The ability to view ourselves from an unbiased perspective allows us to critically assess our personal strengths and weaknesses. This is an important step in the process of finding the right resources for our personal learning style. Ego and pride can be
· By Day 1 of this week
While you must form your answers to the questions below from our assigned reading material
CliftonLarsonAllen LLP (2013)
5 The family dynamic is awkward at first since the most outgoing and straight forward person in the family in Linda
Urien
The most important benefit of my statistical analysis would be the accuracy with which I interpret the data. The greatest obstacle
From a similar but larger point of view
4 In order to get the entire family to come back for another session I would suggest coming in on a day the restaurant is not open
When seeking to identify a patient’s health condition
After viewing the you tube videos on prayer
Your paper must be at least two pages in length (not counting the title and reference pages)
The word assimilate is negative to me. I believe everyone should learn about a country that they are going to live in. It doesnt mean that they have to believe that everything in America is better than where they came from. It means that they care enough
Data collection
Single Subject Chris is a social worker in a geriatric case management program located in a midsize Northeastern town. She has an MSW and is part of a team of case managers that likes to continuously improve on its practice. The team is currently using an
I would start off with Linda on repeating her options for the child and going over what she is feeling with each option. I would want to find out what she is afraid of. I would avoid asking her any “why” questions because I want her to be in the here an
Summarize the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psychological research (Comp 2.1) 25.0\% Summarization of the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psych
Identify the type of research used in a chosen study
Compose a 1
Optics
effect relationship becomes more difficult—as the researcher cannot enact total control of another person even in an experimental environment. Social workers serve clients in highly complex real-world environments. Clients often implement recommended inte
I think knowing more about you will allow you to be able to choose the right resources
Be 4 pages in length
soft MB-920 dumps review and documentation and high-quality listing pdf MB-920 braindumps also recommended and approved by Microsoft experts. The practical test
g
One thing you will need to do in college is learn how to find and use references. References support your ideas. College-level work must be supported by research. You are expected to do that for this paper. You will research
Elaborate on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study 20.0\% Elaboration on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study is missing. Elaboration on any potenti
3 The first thing I would do in the family’s first session is develop a genogram of the family to get an idea of all the individuals who play a major role in Linda’s life. After establishing where each member is in relation to the family
A Health in All Policies approach
Note: The requirements outlined below correspond to the grading criteria in the scoring guide. At a minimum
Chen
Read Connecting Communities and Complexity: A Case Study in Creating the Conditions for Transformational Change
Read Reflections on Cultural Humility
Read A Basic Guide to ABCD Community Organizing
Use the bolded black section and sub-section titles below to organize your paper. For each section
Losinski forwarded the article on a priority basis to Mary Scott
Losinksi wanted details on use of the ED at CGH. He asked the administrative resident