week 5 and6 - Biology
G E N E R A L A R T I C L E The Role of Social Cognitive Theory in Farm-to-School-Related Activities: Implications for Child Nutrition* LINDA BERLIN, PhDa,b KIMBERLY NORRIS, PhDc JANE KOLODINSKY, PhDd,e ABBIE NELSON, MSf ABSTRACT BACKGROUND: Farm-to-school (FTS) programs are gaining attention for many reasons, one of which is the recognition that they could help stem the increase in childhood overweight and obesity. Most FTS programs that have been evaluated have increased students’ selection or intake of fruits and vegetables following the incorporation of FTS components. However, the wide range of activities that are typically part of FTS programs make it difficult to pinpoint which components have the greatest potential to improve students’ health behaviors. Within the field of nutrition education, theory-based interventions that target the key underlying factors influencing health behavior offer the most promise. METHODS: We review existing research on dietary health impacts and implications of 3 key FTS-related activities and explore the component activities of FTS in terms of their potential to address the key constructs of social cognitive theory (SCT) — which is a current best practice in the field of nutrition — suggesting that FTS programs incorporating a diverse set of activities appear to be most promising. RESULTS: We find that components of FTS programs incorporate many of the key theoretical constructs in SCT, and show that FTS programs have great potential to facilitate movement toward desired dietary changes. However, it is unlikely that a set of activities in any one current FTS program addresses multiple constructs of the theory in a systematic manner. CONCLUSION: More intentional inclusion of diverse activities would likely be beneficial. Future research can test these assertions. Keywords: nutrition and diet; school food services; health educators; school health instruction. Citation: Berlin L, Norris K, Kolodinsky J, Nelson A. The role of social cognitive theory in farm-to-school-related activities: implications for child nutrition. J Sch Health. 2013; 83: 589-595. Received on April 3, 2012 Accepted on August 12, 2012 The farm-to-school (FTS) movement gained tractionduring the 1990s, and then flourished over the next decade, resulting in an estimated 2,000 programs in nearly 9,000 schools across the country by 2008.1 Lacking a precise definition, FTS programs are characterized as linking farmers and K-12 schools with the primary purposes of contributing to nutritious meals and education for youth and better incomes for farmers who market locally. Additional goals include enhancing youth’s appreciation and awareness aExtension Assistant Professor, ([email protected]), Department of Nutrition and Food Sciences, University of Vermont, 109 Carrigan Drive, Burlington, VT 05405-0086. bDirector, ([email protected]), Center for Sustainable Agriculture, University of Vermont, 109 Carrigan Drive, Burlington, VT 05405-0086. cAssociate Agent, ([email protected]), Food Supplement Nutrition Education, University of Maryland, 10632 Little Patuxent Parkway, Suite 435, Columbia, MD 21044. dProfessor and Chair, ([email protected]), Community Development and Applied Economics, University of Vermont, 109 Carrigan Drive, Burlington, VT 05405. eDirector, ([email protected]), Center for Rural Studies, University of Vermont, Morrill Hall Rm. 202, Burlington, VT 05405. f Director, ([email protected]), Vermont Food Education Every Day, NOFA-Vermont, PO Box 697, 14 Pleasant Street, Richmond, VT 05477. Address correspondence to: Linda Berlin, Extension Assistant Professor, ([email protected]), Department of Nutrition and Food Sciences, University of Vermont, 109 Carrigan Drive, Burlington, VT 05405-0086. *Indicates CHES continuing education hours are available. Also available at http://www.ashaweb.org/continuing_education.html of agriculture, food, and nutrition, strengthening local economies, and furthering youth’s sense of connectedness to the community.2,3 As a result of FTS programs’ diverse purposes and grassroots nature, the types of activities they encom- pass vary considerably from program to program. This variety may be due to a view that FTS efforts appear to be best designed from the ground up. Despite this diversity, most FTS programs serve locally produced foods in the school cafeteria,4,5 often highlighting fresh or processed fruits and vegetables (eg, kale, Journal of School Health • August 2013, Vol. 83, No. 8 • © 2013, American School Health Association • 589 squash, tomato sauce), dairy and meat products, eggs, beans, and other value-added items (eg, pesto, granola, cider). In addition to locally sourced food served in the cafeteria, components of FTS activities common to many programs include taste tests, lessons on healthful food choices, farm visits, school gardens, recycling activities, and composting systems. The rapid expansion of FTS has been part of a broader food system relocalization movement in the United States. Other facets of this movement, which may or may not happen in communities with FTS programs, have included the revival of farmers’ mar- kets, development of direct marketing relationships between farmers and restaurant operators, forma- tion of CSAs or ‘‘community supported agriculture,’’ and numerous other connections among producers, processors, distributors, and consumers of food.2 Pro- ponents of the food relocalization movement often cite improved food quality and safety, small-scale food production, biodiversity, resource protection, commu- nity well-being, democratic participation, and regional palates.6 Despite their long list of attributes, the con- cepts of ‘‘local’’ and ‘‘regional,’’ as they apply to food systems, are no more precisely defined than is the term ‘‘farm-to-school.’’ FTS programs are gaining attention for their potential to help stem the increasingly prevalent trend of childhood overweight and obesity. A comparison of National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) data over more than 30 years (1976-1980 and 2009-2010) shows that the prevalence of obesity has increased for children aged 6-11 years, from 6.5\% to 18.0\%; and for those aged 12-19 years from 5.0\% to 18.4\%.7,8 Increased consumption of fruits and veg- etables has been recognized as a successful strategy for reducing overweight and obesity,9 and is of particular interest here because greater access to produce is often a core component of FTS efforts.10 In fact, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) has identified FTS programs as an effective approach to improving student health through healthier school meals as well as nutrition and eco-literacy involving hands-on and out-of-doors experiences.11 Many FTS programs that have been evaluated have increased students’ selection or intake of fruits and veg- etables following the incorporation of FTS components such as including locally grown produce into school meal selections, creating school gardens, and provid- ing classroom-based nutrition education.10 Of 5 studies that also examined FTS participants’ dietary behavior outside of school, 4 found increases in the selection or intake of fruits and vegetables by the children. Another study of primary data from 7 school-based nutrition intervention studies — not necessarily incorporating components of FTS programs — showed a net increase of 0.45 servings of fruits and vegetables per student.12 While there are few specific FTS program evaluations, many of the individual activities that are sometimes part of FTS programs such as school gardens have been researched outside of the FTS context. These identify public health implications, including impacts on nutrition knowledge, food preference, and dietary behaviors. The following sections review existing research on the dietary health impacts and implications of three key FTS-related activities. Then, we explore the component activities of FTS in terms of their potential to address the key constructs of social cognitive theory (SCT) — a health behavior change theory on which nutrition interventions are commonly based — suggesting that FTS programs incorporating a diverse set of activities appear to be most promising in this regard.13 LITERATURE REVIEW Research and evaluations that examined the impacts of food-related activities in the school setting are included in this review if they are typical of the types of activities that are conducted within a FTS context. Because FTS does not have a precise definition, not all activities reviewed here were necessarily linked in the school setting to a defined FTS program. Much of the research cited here was identified through a 2009 review by Joshi et al,1 and augmented with additional research published after that report. The 3 broad areas of focus include classroom-based nutrition education activities, school gardens, and food interventions such as school lunch menu changes and taste tests. Component One: Nutrition Education Interventions for Children One of the most explicit goals of FTS is to improve childhood nutrition. Programs may attempt to increase knowledge and awareness, change attitudes, improve skills, or alter behaviors, to positively impact health measures. Despite great interest in this goal, research specifically designed to identify how FTS nutrition education components influence child nutrition has been limited. Furthermore, it is challenging to compare results across the few studies that have been done, due to the diverse array of approaches that fall under the broad definition of FTS.10 Studies show benefits of combining experiential nutrition education with that based in the class- room. Classroom-based nutrition education programs yielded increases in fruit and vegetable consumption among students, from 0.20 to 0.99 servings.14,15 A study of classroom-based nutrition education and hands-on gardening activities for fourth grade students showed that compared with a control group, both these activities ‘‘significantly improved the nutrition 590 • Journal of School Health • August 2013, Vol. 83, No. 8 • © 2013, American School Health Association knowledge of the students,’’ as long as 6 months after the intervention. Furthermore, while both activities increased students’ preferences for certain vegetables, the garden-based nutrition education did so for a greater number of vegetables.16 Component Two: School Gardens A review of 11 studies showed that garden- based nutrition interventions have varying impacts on youth’s produce consumption. Of the featured studies, conducted between 1990 and 2007, 5 were school- based, involving children ages 5-15. Of the 4 studies that looked at actual changes in fruit and vegetable intake, 3 found evidence of increased intake. Of the 6 studies that considered fruit and vegetable preferences, 2 showed increased preferences. Of the 3 studies that examined willingness to taste fruits and vegetables, 2 reported increased willingness to taste.17 More recent research has provided clearer evidence that garden-based education improves youth’s appre- ciation of fresh produce. Garden education programs have been shown to improve attitudes toward fruits and vegetables for second to fifth graders.18 In addi- tion, a 12-week pilot intervention for fourth to sixth graders through a summer YMCA program showed increases in the variety of fruits and vegetables ‘‘ever eaten,’’ as well as improvements in vegetable prefer- ences and requests for fruits and vegetables at home.19 A 28-week study of second graders in a school setting showed that youths involved in gardening in addi- tion to classroom-based nutrition education were more likely to choose and consume vegetables in the cafe- teria, as compared with a control group and another group who only received classroom-based nutrition education. The group that participated in gardening activities also showed improved nutrition knowledge and taste ratings compared with the control group.20 Although these studies’ results are promising, their limitations — including small sample sizes, lack of long-term follow-up, convenience samples, and frequent absence of control groups — prevent firm overall conclusions about school gardens’ impacts on youth’s consumption of and preferences for fresh fruits and vegetables. Component Three: School Lunch Option, Taste Tests, and Farm Connections School-based interventions to improve nutrition often incorporate some combination of taste tests in the classroom or cafeteria, more healthful food choices available in the cafeteria, and connections between children and local farmers. Given prominent goals to improve children’s suboptimal intake of fruits and vegetables, such as those put forth in the most recent Child Nutrition Reauthorization Act, much of the recent research on the impact of school food interventions focuses on produce consumption. Seven studies featured in a review of FTS program evaluations showed that participation in FTS programs increased cafeteria offerings of fruits and vegetables. Subsequently, participating students chose more fruits and vegetables than they did before their participation in the programs.1 One study in this review reported that on days when the salad bar was available, approx- imately 85\% of students selected fruits and vegetables, compared to 35\% choosing fruits and vegetables when only hot lunch was available. Between 80\% and 90\% of the salad bar produce that students selected was unprocessed, compared to only 10\% to 20\% of the hot lunch fruits and vegetables they chose.21 Similar studies in Compton, California, showed that students choosing foods from FTS salad bar lunches selected between 90\% and 140\% of United States Department of Agriculture (USDA)-recommended daily servings of fruits and vegetables, while students choosing hot lunch selected just 40\% to 60\% of the recommended servings. For comparison, both groups took close to the recommended amounts of proteins and grains; this suggests that gains in fruit and vegetable consumption may be a unique contribution of school lunch salad bars.22 Across the nation, salad bar lunches consistently offer nearly twice the servings of fruits and vegetables that hot lunches provide.21 In Oregon, FTS salad bar programs raised the average servings of fruits and vegetables taken by students from 1.24 to 2.26.23 In Los Angeles schools, students self-reported eating an average 4.09 daily servings of fruits and vegetables when participating in salad bar lunch programs, com- pared to 2.97 daily servings prior to the introduction of the salad bar. Students selecting the salad bar also reported consuming fewer total daily calories, cholesterol and fats.24 Pennsylvania parents reported that their children opted for healthier foods at home when participating in FTS interventions, specifically noting that they ate fewer foods high in fats and salt.25 FTS programs can be a boon to school lunch programs’ revenue, as studies show that they typically increase school meal participation rates between 4\% and 16\%.26-28 This is an important part of school food services’ budgets, because they must cover part of their operating costs through sales of full-price meals. They are also reimbursed by the USDA per meal served for purchases of commodity foods, as well as for serving free and reduced-price meals to eligible students.29 One California school meal cost analysis showed that participation rate increases of as little as 8\% can offset additional costs of labor related to an FTS salad bar program.27 Little research was identified describing how taste tests within FTS programs affect actual student dietary behavior. Practitioners use taste tests to introduce Journal of School Health • August 2013, Vol. 83, No. 8 • © 2013, American School Health Association • 591 students to nutritious food choices, to educate students about what makes food healthy, and to allow food service providers to assess the feasibility of serving new foods.30 The link between taste tests and actual food choice and intake has yet to be made. Clinical research conducted in the United Kingdom demonstrates that exposure to a vegetable through 8 daily taste tests increased children’s preferences for that vegetable more effectively than reward methods, when compared with control groups.31 Taste tests held in a school setting in Burlington, Vermont, facilitated the integration of new, healthy food items into the school lunch menus, including pesto pasta and pesto pizza, chicken caesar salads, minestrone soup, and granola-yogurt parfaits.32 School food service professionals interviewed in a set of case studies observed that students are more willing to eat fresh fruits and vegetables if they have interacted with the farmer who grew them, through activities such as field trips to the farm or visits by the farmer to the school.33 These qualitative observations warrant further research into the effects that educational interactions with farmers have on students’ consumption of fresh fruits and vegetables. Overall, research has shown these are positive gains from the implementation of FTS programs. However, given the variety of interventions across FTS programs and the grassroots efforts by communities to imple- ment them, FTS programs consist of interventions in search of a theory that provides a conceptual frame- work upon which to build testable hypotheses. Given that FTS programs require resources to implement, having impact research to show the efficacy is an important next step toward policy changes that may help stem the rising tide of childhood obesity. Farm-to-School and Behavior Change Theory Theory-based interventions that target key factors influencing health behavior are a current best practice in the field of nutrition education. The social ecological model is one relevant theory that describes 5 levels on which health-related behaviors and conditions are influenced: intrapersonal, interpersonal, institutional, community, and public policy.34,35 There are multiple theories that address how change might happen at each of these levels of influence, some of which are particularly suited for certain types of interventions. Social cognitive theory, although primarily focused on the interpersonal sphere of influence, also encompasses factors linked to the intrapersonal, institutional, and community levels. This theory is frequently the framework around which youth-related food and nutrition interventions are designed because of its: (1) emphasis on approaches that are important to youth, such as positive reinforcement and (2) applicability to public health issues, as evidenced by its recent applications in these fields.36-38 It is, therefore, a good fit for considering the factors associated with FTS impacting students’ food-related decision-making and behaviors. SCT addresses the relationship among 3 factors that have to do with how people acquire and maintain health-related behaviors: the environment, personal characteristics, and personal experience. These 3 factors operate in a reciprocal manner with each influencing the others, and are translated into a number of specific constructs which can help shape the components of an intervention. For example, an intervention built on SCT might incorporate a changed environment (institutional and community level), positive reinforcements for new behaviors (intrapersonal level), and opportunities to build or enhance behavioral capability (intrapersonal level), self-control (intrapersonal level), and self-efficacy, such as through modeling (interpersonal level).13,39 To understand how the key constructs of SCT relate to FTS activities, the following list provides a basic outline of the constructs 39 and indicates how they might apply to dietary behavior change that incorporates more local, healthful foods: • Behavioral capability: youth having the knowledge and skills that are necessary to choose and consume a diet that incorporates local, healthful foods. • Expectations: youth having beliefs about the likely outcomes of consuming a healthful diet that includes local foods. • Expectancies: youth valuing the results of eating a diet consisting of healthful, local foods. • Locus of control: youth’s perception of who reinforces continued consumption of local, healthful foods. • Reciprocal determinism: interaction between a youth and his or her environment that results in consumption of more healthful, local foods. • Reinforcement: a youth’s response related to the con- sumption of local, healthful foods that increase the chance of the behavior being repeated; reinforce- ment can be provided internally (by oneself) or externally (by another); • Self-control or self-regulation: youth gaining control by monitoring and adjusting personal behaviors (consumption of local, healthful foods). • Self-efficacy: youths’ confidence in their ability to consume local, healthful foods. • Emotional coping response: how youth deal with the sources of anxiety that surround their consumption of local, healthful foods. DISCUSSION Vermont Food Education Every Day (VT FEED) provides a concrete example of how SCT can be incor- porated into FTS programs. Many FTS programs in 592 • Journal of School Health • August 2013, Vol. 83, No. 8 • © 2013, American School Health Association Vermont have been initiated or supported by VT FEED, which encourages and provides technical assistance to schools through its ‘‘3 Cs’’ model: classroom, cafeteria, and community. Schools with the most comprehen- sive programs — those that incorporate all 3 Cs in their efforts to improve their food environments — would likely touch upon the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and community spheres of influence addressed in the social ecological model and SCT. Although VT FEED did not intentionally design its interventions around health behavior-change theory, and no peer-reviewed research yet considers FTS programs in this light, the rest of this paper explores how components of FTS programs in Vermont do (or do not) address key con- structs of SCT, and discusses these programs’ likelihood of influencing long-term health behavior change. Table 1 provides a description of the types of activities that are often incorporated into FTS programs. It then describes whether each activity takes place in the classroom, cafeteria, or community, and which, if any, of the constructs of SCT are addressed when the activity is carried out. As noted previously, the activities incorporated into each FTS program are not consistent across programs, making it impossible to draw a conclusion about the extent to which the constructs of SCT are addressed through FTS. However, some generalizations can be made from the content of Table 1. First, the activities that are commonly part of FTS do touch upon many of the theoretical constructs in SCT, and often an activity has the potential to address a number of constructs. Second, and most importantly, FTS programs are likely to modify the students’ food environment while simultaneously providing opportunities for them to learn through observation of others (modeling) during activities such as taste tests, eating in the cafeteria, gardening, and cooking.30 Together these approaches have great potential to facilitate movement toward desired dietary change. However, more research is needed to test these assertions. Another question to guide further research is whether there are any approaches covered in SCT that could be integrated more consistently into FTS programs. Specific questions to ask toward this end include: Is parental involvement in FTS programs adequate to reinforce key messages at home, and thereby help establish a stronger sense of control (‘‘locus of control’’) in students? How can empirical evidence be used to assess the value of local foods in nutrition education to justify use of the classroom setting to develop expectations and expectancies? and Are students provided with positive reinforcement when they make nutritionally beneficial choices in the cafeteria? Further research that explores the links between FTS and behavior change theory will enable a closer examination of some of these questions. Ta b le 1 . M at ch in g S o ci al C o g n it iv e T h e o ry to FT S A ct iv it ie s A ct iv it y C la ss ro o m ,C af e te ri a, o r C o m m u n it y? S o ci al C o g n it iv e T h e o ry C o n st ru ct P ri m ar y W ay s th e A ct iv it y H e lp s C ar ry O u t th e C o n st ru ct s Ta st e te st s C la ss ro om or ca fe te ria Po si tiv e re in fo rc em en ts ;e xp ec ta nc ie s; se lf- ef fic ac y; m od el in g/ ob se rv at io na l le ar ni ng ;e m ot io na lc op in g re sp on se St ud en ts le ar n th at ea tin g pr od uc e ca n be en jo ya bl e. Th is in cr ea se s th e ch an ce th ey w ill tr y so m et hi ng in th e fu tu re (‘‘ ex pe ct an ci es ’’) ;a du lts w ho pa rt ic ip at e in ta st in g ac tiv iti es ar e ab le to gi ve yo ut h ku do s fo r tr yi ng ne w fo od s (‘‘ po si tiv e re in fo rc em en t’’ )a nd m od el ta st in g ne w fo od s (‘‘ m od el in g’ ’); yo ut h w ho tr y an d lik e ne w fo od s ga in co nfi de nc e ab ou tt he ir ab ili ty to tr y ne w fo od s in th e fu tu re (‘‘ se lf- ef fic ac y’ ’); st ud en ts w ho ar e fe ar fu lo ft ry in g ne w fo od s m ay ov er co m e th is an xi et y w he n th ey ar e ex po se d to th e po si tiv e re sp on se of th ei rp ee rs (‘‘ em ot io na lc op in g re sp on se ’’) St ud en ts he lp de si gn ,b ui ld ,a nd te nd sc ho ol ga rd en s O ut do or ‘‘c la ss ro om ’’ Be ha vi or al ca pa bi lit y; re ci pr oc al de te rm in is m M or e kn ow le dg e an d sk ill s ab ou tg ar de ni ng in cr ea se s in te re st in th e en d pr od uc t( ‘‘r ec ip ro ca l de te rm in is m ’’) ;L ea rn in g ne w ga rd en in g sk ill s bu ild s ab ili tie s (’b eh av io ra lc ap ab ili ty ) Lo ca lf oo ds in th e ca fe te ria Ca fe te ria Se lf- ef fic ac y; lo cu s of co nt ro l; re ci pr oc al de te rm in is m In cr ea se d av ai la bi lit y in th e ca fe te ria gi ve s st ud en ts th e ch an ce to tr y th in gs in de pe nd en tly (‘‘ lo cu s of co nt ro l’’) ;s el ec tin g ne w ite m s in th e ca fe te ria en ab le s yo ut h to ex pe rie nc e su cc es s th at ca n re su lt in a se ns e of m as te ry (‘‘ se lf- ef fic ac y’ ’) N ut rit io n ed uc at io n in th e cl as sr oo m Cl as sr oo m Po si tiv e re in fo rc em en ts ;e xp ec ta tio ns ; se lf- ef fic ac y M or e fo od -r el at ed kn ow le dg e an d sk ill s in cr ea se s in te re st in th e pr od uc e (‘‘ ex pe ct at io ns ’’) ;t ea ch er s ar e ab le to co m m en d st ud en ts fo rt he ir ga in s (‘‘ po si tiv e re in fo rc em en ts ’’) w hi ch le ad s to a gr ea te rs en se of ab ili ty to m ak e go od fo od -r el at ed de ci si on s (‘‘ se lf- ef fic ac y’ ’) Sa la d ba rt ra in in g fo rs tu de nt s Ca fe te ria Be ha vi or al ca pa bi lit y; St ud en ts re co gn iz e th ey ar e ca pa bl e of se le ct in g he al th fu lc ho ic es (‘‘ be ha vi or al ca pa bi lit y’ ’) Lo ca lf ar m vi si ts Co m m un ity Re ci pr oc al de te rm in is m M or e in te ra ct io n w ith th e fa rm an d fa rm er re su lts in m or e in te re st in he al th fu lc ho ic es (‘‘ re ci pr oc al de te rm in is m ’’) In -c la ss fo od pr ep ar at io n an d sh ar in g Ca fe te ria Be ha vi or al ca pa bi lit y; ex pe ct at io ns ; St ud en ts re co gn iz e th ey ar e ca pa bl e of pr ep ar in g he al th fu lc ho ic es (‘‘ be ha vi or al ca pa bi lit y’ ’); fo od pr ep ar at io n le ad s to in cr ea se d in te re st in tr yi ng ne w fo od s an d ho pe th at it w ill be ta st y (‘‘ ex pe ct at io ns ’’) F T S ,f ar m to sc h o o l. Journal of School Health • August 2013, Vol. 83, No. 8 • © 2013, American School Health Association • 593 FTS programs that incorporate a number of diverse activities are apt to lead to positive dietary behavior change, in part because of their capacity to address multiple constructs of SCT. However, because most FTS programs are locally driven and differ from place to place,5 they do not necessarily combine multiple activities using SCT or … SCT and SEM Social Cognitive Theory & Socio ecological model 1 Social Cognitive Theory 2 What is the Social Cognitive Theory? Theoretical perspective focusing on: - individual characteristics (i.e., self-efficacy); - how people learn by observing others; - how the environment influences behavior. Created by Albert Bandura (Ormrod, 2008) (http://webspace.ship.edu/cgboer/bandura.html) 3 Changing behavior According to the (SCT), changing a behavior depends on two factors: Individual Characteristics (internal) Environmental Factors (external) Within each of these factors there are several constructs (Edberg, 2007 ) 4 SCT Categories & Concepts Reciprocal determinism Psychological determinants of behavior Outcome Expectations Self-efficacy Collective efficacy Observational Learning Environmental determinants of behavior Incentive motivation Facilitation Self-regulation (Excellent example from Cookie Monster) Moral disengagement 5 Reciprocal determinism “Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) describes learning in terms of the interrelationship between behavior, environmental factors, and personal factors.” (IDEA , 2009) 6 the Reciprocal effect to change (PB) a persons thoughts and beliefs, can affect a person’s behavior. For example, one’s personal self efficacy beliefs about writing an essay can influence one’s writing behaviors, such as choice of literary topics, effort, and persistence. (PE) Self-efficacy beliefs also can affect a person’s environment; for example, efficacious students who are trying to write in a noisy social or physical environment may increase their personal concentration to avoid distractions. (EP) Conversely, one’s social environment can affect personal variables and behaviors. Students who receive encouraging feedback from teachers may feel more personally efficacious and work harder to succeed. Teachers can inspire students to write by creating a favorable classroom environment, such as by giving children adequate time to write and revise. (BP)The influence of behavior on personal variables can be seen in the student who succeeds in reading a moderately difficult book and then experiences higher self-efficacy and motivation to try reading another book of comparable difficulty. (BE) Behaviors also can affect the environment, as when students eliminate distractions from their environments (e.g., turn off the television so they can read better). 7 Incentive Motivation The use of rewards and punishment to modify behavior. Reinforcements Specify behaviors you will reinforce. Provide people with clear feedback. Punishments Rules and consequences posted Consistency (e.g., zero tolerance policy) Reinforcements: I really like the way Kevin is working quietly. Feedback: Explain why student received full credit so they can repeat this behavior. Consistency: Students are less likely to break a rule when seeing classmate reprimanded. 8 Self-regulatIon “Controlling oneself though self-monitoring, goal setting, feedback, self reward, self-instruction and enlistment of social support.” Examples Set goals Monitor progress toward the goals Assess the extent to which goals are met Behavior change assessment Food diary Health assessment Problem solving Self-management (Edberg, 2007 p.54) some programs that aid in Self-regulation AA/NA, Weight Watchers, Big Brothers/Big Sisters 9 Outcome Expectations Beliefs about the likelihood (expectations) and value (expectancies) of the consequences of behavioral choices. Expectations: what someone thinks will happen if they make a change. Expectancies: whether the person thinks the expected outcome is good or likely to be rewarded. Do the Expectations and Expectancies sub-constructs look familiar? Expectations Analogous to: Behavioral Beliefs Expectancies Analogous to: Evaluations of Behavioral Outcomes These are the constricts that influence attitude towards behavior in the TRA. 10 Self-Efficacy “A person’s confidence that he or she can perform a behavior.” Self-Efficacy vs. Self-Esteem Confidence to perform a skill vs. how one feels about him/herself. A child first learning to ride a bike may not feel very confident. A child with high self-efficacy in physical activity would attempt to ride a bike, even when feeling unsure, because they have mastered other difficult tasks in the past. With low self-efficacy, the child may be hesitant to pedal a bike for fear that he or she may fall. (Edberg, 2007, p. 53) Mastery Experience Practice Social Modeling Others can do it and so can you! Example? Saw a friend do a half-marathon… 11 Collective Efficacy “Beliefs about the ability of a group to perform concerted actions that bring desired outcomes.” Example: MADD’s ability to create new alcohol related laws 12 Vicarious learning A person learns by observing the behavior of others and the consequences of that behavior. Watch the Bobo the clown experiment in blackboard content BoBo Doll Experiment http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Pr0OTCVtHbU (Edberg, 2007) 1:22 Observational Learning Learning behaviors by exposure to interpersonal or media displays of them, particularly through peer modeling. 14 Facilitation Providing tools, resources, or environmental changes that make new behaviors easier to perform. Example: greenways to facilitate walking/bike riding 15 Moral disengagement Ways of thinking about harmful behaviors and the people who are harmed that make infliction of suffering acceptable by disengaging self-regulatory moral standards. Rationalize, justify, or permit conditions which compromise health. Example: allowing vape companies to market towards youth. Business rights vs. health of children Euphemistic labeling Consider homophobia, racism, sexism, etc. Examples? Dehumanization and attribution of blame Drug testing welfare recipients? Diffusion and displacement of responsibility Fault of the leader, not the individual? Perceived moral justification Ends justify the means? 16 Limitations “SCT can’t explain why learners can reproduce some behaviors they observe but can’t reproduce others.” (Eggen & Kauchak, 2010, p.188) 17 Limitations “SCT cannot explain the role of context and social interaction in complex learning environments.” “For example, student interaction in small groups facilitates learning. The process involved in these settings extend beyond simple modeling and imitation.” (Eggen & Kauchak, 2010 p.188) 18 So What? You personally, your behavior, and your environment all affect each other. People may have trouble changing their health behavior because of what is going on around them so look at the “big” picture. Be careful what you do because you never know who is watching. Children see, children do video 19 Socio ecological model History: Ecological Model Until the late 1970s/early 1980s, health promotion professionals focused primarily on the knowledge, and attitudes of individuals. There was an overall lack of attention devoted to the social, cultural, and economic circumstances which influence behavior. (Edberg, 2007) 21 Why the Ecological Model is different? Assumed that multiple factors influences peoples’ behaviors rather than one cause. i.e., Lack of knowledge It is a complex interaction between individuals and environment, a process that together influences behavior The Ecological Model takes into consideration community, society, and relationships as potential contributors to the behavior of individuals 22 Ecological Model Explain Behavior Different Levels: Environmental Political Socioeconomic Cultural You! 23 Ecological Model Guide Interventions 24 1st Level: Individual Factors Intrapersonal Awareness and knowledge about health risks, ways to prevent health problems, etc. Biophysical characteristics Genetics Personal attitudes and motivations Developmental stage Adolescent, adult Habits (Edberg, 2007) 25 2nd Level: Interpersonal The extent to which relationships and social networks influence behavior. Social/peer groups impact lifestyle patterns Attitudes/beliefs/behaviors of people the individual interacts with Consider college student behavior… 26 3rd Level: Organizational Organizations and social institutions impact health. Campuses, class schedules, workplace, childcare, etc. (Edberg, 2007) 27 4th Level: Community The effect the community has on health behavior. Walkability, businesses, transportation, etc. 28 5th Level: Public Policy How state and federal laws affect people’s health behaviors Policies and funding for HPP Health insurance (policies, costs, availability) Regulations that impact health risk (such as age regulations) Smoking, drinking, driving laws, etc. (Edberg, 2007) 29 Limitations How easy it is to address each stage of the Ecological Model? What about resources ($$) needed? Time? Compare it to, say, the Health Belief Model… references Anderson, E., Winett, R., & Wojcik, J. (2007, November). Self-Regulation, Self-Efficacy, Outcome Expectations, and Social Support: Social Cognitive Theory and Nutrition Behavior. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 34(3), 304-312. Retrieved September 29, 2009, doi:10.1080/08836610701677659 Edberg Mark, (2007) Essentials of Health Behavior. “Social Cognitive Theory” Chapter 5 p. 51-56, Jones and Bartlett Publishers. Washington, DC. Eggen, P. & Kauchak, D. (2010) Educational Psychology. “Social Cognitive Theory” Chapter 6 p. 179-188, Merrill. Columbus, OH. Institute for Dynamic Educational Advancement (IDEA). Journal of Interaction Recipes, (2009) “Social Cognitive Theory” Retrieved: Sep. 29, 2009. http://www.idea.org/page110.html Schunk, D., & Zimmerman, B. (2007). Influencing Childrens Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulation of Reading and Writing Through Modeling. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 23(1), 7-25. Retrieved September 29, 2009, doi:10.1080/10573560600837578 Glanz, Karen; Rimer, Barbara K.; Lewis, Frances Marcus. Health Behavior and Health Education theory, research, and practice. Chapter 8 pgs. 168-181. San-Francisco: Jossey-Bass 2002. Print Martin, J.J. (2008). Using Social Cognitive Theory to Predict Physical Activity in Inner- City African American School Children. Journal of Sport and Exercise Physiology, 30 (4), 378-391. Ormrod, Jeanne Ellis (2008). Educational Psychology. Chapter 10 pg. 343. Pearson Education Inc. New Jersey. 31 Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wagr20 Journal of Agromedicine ISSN: 1059-924X (Print) 1545-0813 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wagr20 Using the Socio-Ecological Model to Frame Agricultural Safety and Health Interventions Barbara C. Lee, Casper Bendixsen, Amy K. Liebman & Susan S. Gallagher To cite this article: Barbara C. Lee, Casper Bendixsen, Amy K. Liebman & Susan S. Gallagher (2017) Using the Socio-Ecological Model to Frame Agricultural Safety and Health Interventions, Journal of Agromedicine, 22:4, 298-303, DOI: 10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 Published online: 29 Aug 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 1912 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 7 View citing articles https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wagr20 https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wagr20 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 https://doi.org/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=wagr20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=wagr20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-08-29 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-08-29 https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780#tabModule https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780#tabModule COMMENTARIES Using the Socio-Ecological Model to Frame Agricultural Safety and Health Interventions Barbara C. Leea, Casper Bendixsena, Amy K. Liebmanb, and Susan S. Gallagherc aNational Children’s Center for Rural and Agricultural Health and Safety, Marshfield Clinic, Marshfield, Wisconsin, USA; bEnvironmental and Occupational Health, Migrant Clinicians Network, Salisbury, Maryland, USA; cPublic Health and Community Medicine, Tufts University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts, USA ABSTRACT The Socio-Ecological Model (SEM) is a conceptual framework depicting spheres of influence over human behavior that has been applied in public health settings for nearly five decades. Core principles of all variations of the SEM are the multiple influences over an individual’s behaviors, the interactions of those influences, and the multilevel approaches that can be applied to interventions intended to modify behaviors. A project team modified the standard SEM to address interventions for protecting children from agricultural disease and injury. The modified SEM placed the “child in the farm environment” at the core with five interrelated levels (spheres) of influence over the child. This framework provides guidance on how a multifaceted, multilevel intervention can maximize the potential for impact on behaviors and decisions made by parents/ adults responsible for the safety of children on farms. An example of how this model could work to safeguard youth operating tractors is provided. KEYWORDS Agriculture; safety; socio- ecological model; theory Background Occupational safety and health advocates are con- stantly searching for strategies that offer sustain- able interventions that reduce risks of injury and disease. These strategies are often based on educa- tion, engineering, environmental, and/or enforce- ment approaches. To strengthen and potentially measure their impact, they can be based on prin- ciples of safety and hygiene, past experience, and sometimes a theoretical model. Agricultural safety and health interventions have lagged behind other occupational safety and public health approaches but increasingly are adopting evidence-based stra- tegies guided by theories and models that have demonstrated success in changing unsafe tradi- tions into safe behaviors. This paper describes how a well-known public health model has been modified for agricultural safety and health to mul- tiply and maximize the impact of agricultural safety interventions. Introduced in the 1970s, the Socio-Ecological Model (SEM) is a broad-based conceptual model depicting basic ecological principles of human behavior.1 The SEM has undergone numerous updates and modifications for different applications.2 The World Health Organization and U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention are among the many users of this model, which illustrates multiple dimensions and complex human interactions that influence behaviors.3,4 At the core of the model is an individual whose beha- vior is the primary interest. A figure of enlarging circles added above the core individual demon- strates how spheres of increasing influence have higher degrees of impact on individual behavior (Figure 1). The next level of influence is his/her interpersonal relationships such as relatives, peer groups, or healthcare providers. Following this is the organizational level, which includes organiza- tions, schools, churches, and workplaces. Next is the community level, which represents relationships between organizations. Finally, at the outer sphere of the figure, is the public policy level that includes federal/state regulations with enforcement options. Terminology for the middle levels of the model is CONTACT Barbara C. Lee [email protected] National Children’s Center for Rural and Agricultural Health and Safety, Marshfield Clinic, 1000 N. Oak Avenue, Marshfield, WI 54449, USA. Color versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found online at www.tandfonline.com/wagr. JOURNAL OF AGROMEDICINE 2017, VOL. 22, NO. 4, 298–303 https://doi.org/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780 © 2017 Taylor & Francis http://www.tandfonline.com/wagr https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/1059924X.2017.1356780&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-08-21 typically altered depending on the user’s needs and the model’s application. In an extensive review of various ecological models of health behaviors published in 2008, authors explain that the core principles of an eco- logical model are: (1) there are multiple influences on an individual’s behaviors, including factors at the intrapersonal level, interpersonal level, with increasing influence at levels of organization, com- munity, and public policy; (2) influences interact across these different levels or spheres of influence; (3) use of this model should be applied to specific behaviors; and (4) multilevel approaches can be the most effective interventions for changing behaviors.5 The evolution of the SEM is based in part on five different theories explaining human behavior, dating from 1951 to 2006, as well as eight different theories used to guide behavior change, dating from 1953 to 2005. The influence of different theorists and their applications of con- ceptual models over time can explain both the strength and the various visual depictions of the SEM for different audiences. Modified model In 1996, the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) launched its National Childhood Agricultural Injury Prevention Initiative.6 As a component of that initiative, the National Children’s Center for Rural and Agricultural Health and Safety (NCCRAHS) was established to link public and private sector initia- tives based upon a national plan of action.7 In 2014, with two decades of experience, the NCCRAHS wanted to base its current and future endeavors on a theoretical model that would max- imize potential impacts. The SEM was chosen as a logical fit for the center’s theme of strengthening public-private partnerships to address childhood agricultural injury prevention.8 The model has long-standing acceptance by public health agen- cies, and it has applications in multiple settings on topics ranging from adding positive nutritional habits and physical activity to avoiding risky prac- tices such as smoking and unsafe sex. To the best of our knowledge, the SEM had not specifically and proactively been applied in agricultural safety and health interventions or program evaluations, nor has it been modified in any specific way to address the well-being of children. A comprehensive review of childhood agricul- tural safety interventions conducted by Gallagher in 20129 assessed 26 peer-reviewed studies that reported the effectiveness of childhood farm safety interventions. It was determined that most inter- ventions focused on the individual level of the SEM and typically used education as the primary strategy to increase knowledge and influence beha- vior change. Based upon these findings, the author provided eight recommendations for the future, framed around the principles of the SEM, such as multilevel partnerships; repeated interventions; approaches beyond education (e.g., engineering, policy); diversity in funding; and sustained, wide- spread dissemination.9 A planning team at the NCCRAHS reviewed literature and versions of the SEM and discussed the impact of the spheres of influence relevant to the political, social, and individual environment affiliated with agricultural communities. The team incorporated concepts from non-agricultural projects including experiences using the model for low-income workers. Our modified version of the SEM (Figure 2) placed the “child in the farm environment” at the core of the figure, with the knowledge that a child (up to 18 years) who lives, visits, or works on a farm is not in a position to change safety practices Figure 1. Socio-ecological model: framework for prevention, centers for disease control. Available from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). http://www.cdc.gov/vio lenceprevention/overview/social-ecologicalmodel.html.4 JOURNAL OF AGROMEDICINE 299 http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/overview/social-ecologicalmodel.html http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/overview/social-ecologicalmodel.html him/herself. Rather, the focus of interventions is to influence the behaviors of those adults who have the authority and knowledge to reduce the risk of injury and disease affecting children. We believe that all children deserve equal protection from preventable disease and injury, and adults hold full responsibility for safeguarding children under their care. At the model’s core is the child under the influence and protection of the adult(s). There are five spheres with interrelated-levels of influ- ence over the child. The adult sphere includes parents, guardian, farm owners, employers, and any other individual(s) who may have responsi- bility for youth in the agricultural production site or a farm homestead. The next level of influence is interpersonal—this includes persons with close relationship to the immediate family such as rela- tives, friends, and peer groups. It can also include health care providers and child care providers who regularly interact with the family. At the third sphere above the child is the community level, which can include local businesses such as farm cooperatives and community-based organi- zations such as FFA chapters, schools, faith-based groups/churches, and child care centers. At a higher level of influence are institutions and orga- nizations that span beyond the local region. This includes agricultural companies such as property/ casualty insurance providers, trade associations, agribusinesses that set standards and guidelines for purchasing agricultural products, national/ international trade agreements, bankers and lend- ing agencies, and national media that influence public opinion. The highest level of influence is policy. For the most part, this represents federal and state regulations regarding the role of youth in agricultural work. It can also represent issues such as immigration, federal/state workers com- pensation laws, and Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA) enforcement standards. Degrees of influence of the various spheres are subject to many factors. Each superordinate level influences the subordinate level. For example, a public policy may influence a community program that influences an adult to make responsible deci- sions regarding work assigned to a child living on a farm. When applying this modified SEM concept to agricultural safety and health interventions, the ideal approach is to have an interrelational link that crosses through as many spheres as possible. We have solid evidence from interventions in non- agricultural settings that a multilevel approach with repeated interventions has the greatest like- lihood of achieving the desired outcome. For example, a 2014 report described how the SEM was used in a multilevel intervention to reduce health inequities among low-income workers.10 Another example is an assessment to propose community outreach interventions to improve Figure 2. Socio-ecological model modified to address agricultural safety and health interventions. 300 B. C. LEE ET AL. fruit and vegetable intake among inner-city African Americans. Literature was reviewed on past interventions addressing this topic. Relevant interventions were categorized by SEM level then, based upon intervention effectiveness, and recom- mendations for a multifaceted community-based approach became the basis and rationale for “Best Practices” ecological nutritional programs for African Americans.11 Applying the SEM for agricultural safety What would an ideal intervention based on this SEM concept look like? For explanatory purposes, consider an unsafe practice that puts youth at high risk of an agriculture-related injury. What is the desired behavior change? And what approach could be used at multiple levels to influence the adults that bear primary responsibility for youth involved in that unsafe practice? Agricultural safety Equipment manufacturers and safety professionals recommend that all tractors used for production activities include basic safety principles of seatbelts and Rollover Protection Structures (ROPS). It has been shown that this safety standard of a tractor being equipped with a seatbelt and ROPS (or enclosed cab) can virtually eliminate tractor- related fatalities when the operator appropriately uses these safety features.12,13 Burden For youth working in agriculture, tractors are the leading cause of death. An analysis of occupational fatality cases from 2001 to 2013 among U.S. work- ers under the age of 18 revealed that of the 406 recorded fatalities across all occupations, about 50\% of deaths occurred in agricultural jobs, of which nearly all were associated with transporta- tion and equipment.14 Young workers are often asked to operate tractors that do not meet safety standards, because the older unsafe tractors may be smaller, less expensive, and less complicated to operate, and farm owners do not want young people operating their high-powered, expensive equipment. There are no child labor regulations that mandate safety standards of tractors operated by youth. Further, in the United States, family farms are exempt from child labor in agriculture regulations. In occupational settings, the parent or work supervisor bears responsibility for ensuring that a young worker is safeguarded. However, agricultural work activities can be complicated, making close supervision and oversight difficult to maintain, especially when workers are doing field operations with tractors and trailed implements. Solution To minimize the toll of serious injuries and deaths among young workers in agriculture, a solution would be to ensure that youth (14–18 years) who are assigned agricultural work involving tractor operations be allowed only to operate tractors equipped with ROPS, and that these youth be required to wear the tractor seatbelt at all times. Implementing this solution would entail a multi- level, integrated approach that alters long-standing practices and might challenge family and/or cul- tural traditions. Applying the SEM to a multilevel, integrated intervention would involve each sphere of influence approaching the problem from a dif- ferent angle, but all with the same desired outcome of improving safety. The scenario below (Table 1) describes an inter- vention, based on the SEM, of a national-level campaign to “Safeguard youth operating tractors.” The scenario above is an idealistic picture of how the SEM could work, involving entities at all levels of the SEM, and proposing they would agree and engage in a unified way. Realistically, this would be time and resource intensive and difficult to exe- cute. But undoubtedly, if this scenario were set into operation, there could be a profound change that would drastically reduce the toll of injuries and deaths to youth operating tractors. Implications Putting the SEM into practice in agriculture is possible. Over the past five decades, much has been learned about the etiology of farm injuries through data on the incidence of injuries and details on changing trends in types of injuries. JOURNAL OF AGROMEDICINE 301 Our biggest challenge moving forward is improving safety interventions and taking approaches that will have the biggest impact on reducing the toll of injuries. These multilevel and interrelated interventions have the potential of shifting the “culture” of agriculture to have a greater emphasis on and respect for a “safety culture” in agriculture. It also broadens the gen- eral public’s perspective on the issue, rather than solely relying on direct interventions by parents or policy-level changes. The diversity of indivi- duals and organizations involved strengthens the capacity to change practices, resulting in lives saved. Conclusions Public health demonstration programs have shown us the SEM is a strong and effective way to change individual behaviors by influencing those beha- viors at multiple levels. We propose to modify the SEM for application in agricultural safety and health promotion programs. As this model is applied, evaluated, and improved over time, our hope is to have a measurable and sustained improvement in safe practices that create a true culture of safety in agriculture. Funding Efforts toward developing this dedicated issue of the Journal of Agromedicine with a focus on the Socio-Ecological Model were funded in part by the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health via the National Children’s Center for Rural and Agricultural Health and Safety (NIOSH 2U54OH009568) and the National Farm Medicine Center. References 1. Brofenbreener U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1979. 2. Images of Socio-ecological model. https://www.google. com/search?q=socio-ecologic+models+cdc+ima ges&tbm=isch&tbo=u&source=univ&sa=X&ved= 0ahUKEwiL67r4mobUAhXp6oMKHYSUCuEQsAQIM g&biw=1152&bih=616. Accessed May 22, 2017. Table 1. An intervention, based on the SEM, of a national-level campaign to “Safeguard youth operating tractors.” Policy Federal child labor laws in agriculture would be changed to set a minimum age of 16 years to operate tractors on public roads and 14 years to operate tractors on private land. The family farm exemption would be eliminated. Federal and state OSHA would establish minimum age limits for all safety standards and would require workers younger than 18 years to wear seatbelts and operate only tractors with ROPS. OSHA standards regarding tractor operations would be enforceable on all farms regardless of number of employees. Institution/organization Tractor manufacturers (e.g., via Association of Equipment Manufacturers [AEM] trade association) would publicly announce a position statement that supports the OSHA standard. Agribusinesses would require compliance with federal/state laws and OSHA standards as an expectation of entities from whom they purchase products. National FFA would set a national standard that their Student Agricultural Experience (SAE) ensure youth are in settings where they comply with this safety standard and announce their position via National FFA communication mechanisms that reach advisors, members, and alumni. Other organizations such as the American Academy of Pediatrics would post a position on this safety standard. The national media would publish stories about this national campaign to protect young tractor operators. Media stories of lives saved would begin to shift traditional thinking about guidelines for young people operating tractors. Community A comprehensive social marketing campaign would be launched to “Safeguard Youth Operating Tractors.” The campaign would be crafted with messages and dissemination strategies based on stakeholder input. Using targeted campaign messages, including social media outlets at the regional and local level, FFA Chapters, schools, and faith- based groups would facilitate efforts of farm owners to ensure any tractors operated by youth are safely equipped. Incentives would be provided by local insurers and bankers, offering economic aid for farmers needing financial assistance to upgrade their tractors operated by youth. These community groups would promulgate the campaign messages and, where appropriate, the position statements issued by national-level organizations. School-based activities would no longer promote “ride your tractor to school” events but would emphasize campaign messages and facilitate tractor safety certification programs. Community-level advocates for the campaign would be trained to deal with controversies surrounding the tractor topic. Interpersonal Peer groups, friends, and relatives would share “Safeguard youth operating tractors” campaign materials and openly encourage farm owners and parents to adopt the recommended practices and OSHA standards. These people would reach out to underserved, hard-to-reach farm owners (e.g., niche farms, special populations) with the same information and expectations regarding youth involved in agricultural work. Adult Farm parents, farm owners, and employers would acknowledge the multilevel pressure being exerted to change farm practices and comply with the new OSHA standard by not allowing youth to operate tractors unsafely. Child/youth Young tractor operators would have strict safety standards set, having access only to ROPS tractors as well as knowing and understanding they are required to wear seatbelts. 302 B. C. LEE ET AL. https://www.google.com/search?q=socio-ecologic+models+cdc+images\%26tbm=isch\%26tbo=u\%26source=univ\%26sa=X\%26ved=0ahUKEwiL67r4mobUAhXp6oMKHYSUCuEQsAQIMg\%26biw=1152\%26bih=616 https://www.google.com/search?q=socio-ecologic+models+cdc+images\%26tbm=isch\%26tbo=u\%26source=univ\%26sa=X\%26ved=0ahUKEwiL67r4mobUAhXp6oMKHYSUCuEQsAQIMg\%26biw=1152\%26bih=616 https://www.google.com/search?q=socio-ecologic+models+cdc+images\%26tbm=isch\%26tbo=u\%26source=univ\%26sa=X\%26ved=0ahUKEwiL67r4mobUAhXp6oMKHYSUCuEQsAQIMg\%26biw=1152\%26bih=616 https://www.google.com/search?q=socio-ecologic+models+cdc+images\%26tbm=isch\%26tbo=u\%26source=univ\%26sa=X\%26ved=0ahUKEwiL67r4mobUAhXp6oMKHYSUCuEQsAQIMg\%26biw=1152\%26bih=616 https://www.google.com/search?q=socio-ecologic+models+cdc+images\%26tbm=isch\%26tbo=u\%26source=univ\%26sa=X\%26ved=0ahUKEwiL67r4mobUAhXp6oMKHYSUCuEQsAQIMg\%26biw=1152\%26bih=616 3. World Health Organization. The ecological framework. 2017. http://www.who.int/violenceprevention/ approach/ecology/en/. Accessed May 22, 2017. 4. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2015. The social-ecological model: a framework for prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/overview/ social-ecologicalmodel.html. Accessed July 3, 2017. 5. Sallis JF, Owen N, Fisher EB. Ecological models of health behavior. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, eds. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice. 4th ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2008:465–485. 6. Castillo D, Hard D, Myers J, Pizatella T, Stout N. A national childhood agricultural injury prevention initiative. J Agric Saf Health. 1998;4:183–191. doi:10.13031/2013.15368. 7. National Committee for Childhood Agricultural Injury Prevention. Children and Agriculture: Opportunities for Safety and Heatlh. Marshfield, WI: Marshfield Clinic; 1996. 8. National Center of Excellence for the Prevention of Childhood Agricultural Injury (RFA: OH-14-005); Grant Application Submitted to National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. Marshfield, WI: Marshfield Clinic Research Foundation; 2014. 9. Gallagher SS. Characteristics of evaluated childhood agricultural safety interventions. J Agromedicine. 2012;17:109–126. doi:10.1080/1059924X.2012.664033. 10. Baron SL, Beard S, Davis LK, et al. Promoting integrated approaches to reducing health inequities among low- income workers: applying a social ecological framework. Am J Ind Med. 2014;57:539–556. doi:10.1002/ajim.v57.5. 11. Robinson T. Applying the socio-ecological model to improving fruit and vegetable intake among low- income African American. J Community Health. 2008;33:395–406. doi:10.1007/s10900-008-9109-5. 12. Springfeldt B, Thorson J, Lee B. Sweden’s thirty-year experience with tractor rollovers. J Agric Saf Health. 1998;4:173–180. doi:10.13031/2013.15355. 13. Reynolds SJ, Goves W. Effectiveness of roll-over pro- tective structure in reducing farm tractor fatalities. Am J Prev Med. 2000;18:63–69. doi:10.1016/S0749-3797(00) 00142-2. 14. Rauscher KJ, Myers DJ. Occupational fatalities among young workers in the United States: 2001-2012. Am J Ind Med. 2016;59:445–452. doi:10.1002/ajim.v59.6. JOURNAL OF AGROMEDICINE 303 http://www.who.int/violenceprevention/approach/ecology/en/ http://www.who.int/violenceprevention/approach/ecology/en/ https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/overview/social-ecologicalmodel.html https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/overview/social-ecologicalmodel.html http://dx.doi.org/10.13031/2013.15368 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1059924X.2012.664033 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajim.v57.5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10900-008-9109-5 http://dx.doi.org/10.13031/2013.15355 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00142-2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00142-2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajim.v59.6 Abstract Background Modified model Applying the SEM for agricultural safety Agricultural safety Burden Solution Implications Conclusions Funding References NIH Publication No. 05-3896 Printed September 2005 2931-NCI Theory cvr.f 11/17/05 3:18 PM Page 1 Theor y at a Gl ance U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES National Institutes of Health A G u i d e F o r H e a l t h P ro m o t i o n P ra c t i ce Theory at a Glance A Guide For Health Promotion Practice (Second Edition) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES National Institutes of Health Foreword A decade ago, the first edition of Theory at a Glance was published. The guide was a welcome resource for public health practitioners seeking a single, concise summary of health behavior theories that was neither overwhelming nor superficial. As a government publication in the public domain, it also provided cash-strapped health departments with access to a seminal integration of scholarly work that was useful to program staff, interns, and directors alike. Although they were not the primary target audience, members of the public health research community also utilized Theory at a Glance, both as a quick desk reference and as a primer for their students. The National Cancer Institute is pleased to sponsor the publication of this guide, but its relevance is by no means limited to cancer prevention and control. The principles described herein can serve as frameworks for many domains of public health intervention, complementing focused evidence reviews such as Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Guide to Community Preventive Services. This report also complements a number of other efforts by NCI and our federal partners to facilitate more rigorous testing and application of health behavior theories through training workshops and the development of new Web-based resources. One reason theory is so useful is that it helps us articulate assumptions and hypotheses concerning our strategies and targets of intervention. Debates among policymakers concerning public health programs are often complicated by unspoken assumptions or confusion about which data are relevant. Theory can inform these debates by clarifying key constructs and their presumed relationships. Especially when the evidence base is small, advocates of one approach or another can be challenged to address the mechanisms by which a program is expected to have an impact. By specifying these alternative pathways to change, program evaluations can be designed to ensure that regardless of the outcome, improvements in knowledge, program design, and implementation will occur. I am pleased to introduce this second edition of Theory at a Glance. I am especially impressed that the lead authors, Dr. Barbara K. Rimer and Dr. Karen Glanz, have enhanced and updated it throughout without diminishing the clarity and efficiency of the original. We hope that this new edition will empower another generation of public health practitioners to apply the same conceptual rigor to program planning and design that these authors exemplify in their own research and practice. Robert T. Croyle, Ph.D. Director Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences National Cancer Institute Spring 2005 Acknowledgements The National Cancer Institute would like to thank Barbara Rimer Dr.P.H. and Karen Glanz Ph.D., M.P.H., authors of the original monograph, whose knowledge of healthcommunications theory and practice have molded a generation of health promotion practitioners. Both have provided hours of review and consultation, and we are grateful to them for their contributions. Thanks to the staffs of the Office of Communications, particularly Margaret Farrell, and the Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences and Kelly Blake, who guided this monograph to completion. We appreciate in particular the work of Karen Harris, whose attention to detail and commitment to excellence enhanced the monograph’s content and quality. Table of Contents Introduction viii Audience and Purpose 1 Contents 1 Part 1: Foundations of Theory in Health Promotion and Health Behavior 3 Why Is Theory Important to Health Promotion and Health Behavior Practice? 4 What Is Theory? 4 How Can Theory Help Plan Effective Programs? 4 Explanatory Theory and Change Theory 5 Fitting Theory to the Field of Practice 5 Using Theory to Address Health Issues in Diverse Populations 7 Part 2: Theories and Applications 9 The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach 10 Theoretical Explanations of Three Levels of Influence 12 Individual or Intrapersonal Level 12 Health Belief Model 13 Stages of Change Model 15 Theory of Planned Behavior 16 Precaution Adoption Process Model 18 Interpersonal Level 19 Social Cognitive Theory 19 Community Level 22 Community Organization and Other Participatory Models 23 Diffusion of Innovations 27 Communication Theory 29 Media Effects 30 Agenda Setting 30 New Communication Technologies 31 Part 3: Putting Theory and Practice Together 35 Planning Models 36 Social Marketing 36 PRECEDE-PROCEED 39 Where to Begin: Choosing the Right Theories 43 A Few Final Words 44 Sources 48 References 49 Tables and Figures Tables Table 1 An Ecological Perspective: Levels of Influence 11 Table 2 Health Belief Model 14 Table 3 Stages of Change Model 15 Table 4 Theory of Planned Behavior 17 Table 5 Social Cognitive Theory 20 Table 6 Community Organization 24 Table 7 Concepts in Diffusion of Innovations 27 Table 8 Key Attributes Affecting the Speed and Extent of an Innovation’s Diffusion 28 Table 9 Agenda Setting, Concepts, Definitions, and Applications 31 Table 10 Diagnostic Elements of PRECEDE-PROCEED 42 Table 11 Summary of Theories: Focus and Key Concepts 45 Figures Figure 1 Using Explanatory Theory and Change Theory to Plan and Evaluate Programs 6 Figure 2 A Multilevel Approach to Epidemiology 10 Figure 3 Theory of Reasoned Action and Theory of Planned Behavior 18 Figure 4 Stages of the Precaution Adoption Process Model 19 Figure 5 An Integrative Model 21 Figure 6 Sociocultural Environment Logic Framework 26 Figure 7 An Asthma Self-Management Video Game for Children 33 Figure 8 Social Marketing Wheel 38 Figure 9 The PRECEDE-PROCEED Model 40 Figure 10 Using Theory to Plan Multilevel Interventions 46 Introduction viii T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E T his monograph, Theory at a Glance: Application to Health Promotion and Health Behavior (Second Edition), describes influential theories of health-related behaviors, processes of shaping behavior, and the effects of community and environmental factors on behavior. It complements existing resources that offer tools, techniques, and model programs for practice, such as Making Health Communication Programs Work: A Planner’s Guide,i and the Web portal, Cancer Control PLANET (Plan, Link, Act, Network with Evidence-based Tools).ii Theory at a Glance makes health behavior theory accessible and provides tools to solve problems and assess the effectiveness of health promotion programs. (For the purposes of this monograph, health promotion is broadly defined as the process of enabling people to increase control over, and to improve, their health. Thus, the focus goes beyond traditional primary and secondary prevention programs.) For nearly a decade, public health and health care practitioners have consulted the original version of Theory at a Glance for guidance on using theories about human behavior to inform program planning, implementation, and evaluation. We have received many testimonials about the First Edition’s usefulness, and requests for additional copies. This updated edition includes information from recent health behavior research and suggests theoretical approaches to developing programs for diverse populations. Theory at a Glance can be used as a stand-alone handbook, as part of in-house staff development programs, or in conjunction with theory texts and continuing education workshops. For easy reference, the monograph includes only a small number of current and applicable health behavior theories. The theories reviewed here are widely used for the purposes of cancer control, defining risk, and segmenting populations. Much of the content for this publication has been adapted from the third edition of Glanz, Rimer, and Lewis’ Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice,1 published by Jossey-Bass in San Francisco. Readers who want to learn more about useful theories for health behavior change and health education practice can consult this and other sources that are recommended in the References section at the end of the monograph. i Making Health Communication Programs Work (http://www.nci.nih.gov/pinkbook/) describes a practical approach for planning and implementing health communication efforts. ii Cancer Control PLANET (http://cancercontrolplanet.cancer.gov) provides access to data and resources that can help planners, program staff, and researchers to design, implement, and evaluate evidence-based cancer control programs. (http://www.nci.nih.gov/pinkbook/) (http://cancercontrolplanet.cancer.gov) Audience and Purpose This monograph is written primarily for public health workers in state and local health agencies; it is also valuable for health promotion practitioners and volunteers who work in voluntary health agencies, community organizations, health care settings, schools, and the private sector. Interventions based on health behavior theory are not guaranteed to succeed, but they are much more likely to produce desired outcomes. Theory at a Glance is designed to help users understand how individuals, groups, and organizations behave and change—knowledge they can use to design effective programs. For information about specific, evidence-based interventions to promote health and prevent disease, readers may also wish to consult the Guide to Community Preventive Services, published by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) at www.thecommunityguide.org. Contents This monograph consists of three parts. For each theory, the text highlights key concepts and their applications. These summaries may be used as “checklists” of important issues to consider when planning or evaluating programs or to prompt project teams to think about the range of factors that influence health behavior. Part 1. Foundations of Theory in Health Promotion and Health Behavior describes ways that theories and models can be useful in health behavior/health promotion practice and provides basic definitions. Part 2. Theories and Applications presents an ecological perspective on health behavior/health promotion programs. It describes eight theories and models that explain individual, interpersonal, and community behavior and offers approaches to solving problems. A brief description of each theory is followed by definitions of key concepts and examples or case studies. The section also explores the use of new communication technologies. Part 3. Putting Theory and Practice Together explains how theory can be used in health behavior/health promotion program planning, implementation, and evaluation. Two comprehensive planning models, PRECEDE-PROCEED and social marketing, are reviewed. 1 IN TR O T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E http:www.thecommunityguide.org Part 1 Foundations of Theory in Health Promotion and Health Behavior 3 PA R T 1 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E 4 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E Why Is Theory Important to Health Promotion and Health Behavior Practice? Effective public health, health promotion, and chronic disease management programs help people maintain and improve health, reduce disease risks, and manage chronic illness. They can improve the well-being and self-sufficiency of individuals, families, organizations, and communities. Usually, such successes require behavior change at many levels, (e.g., individual, organizational, and community). Not all health programs and initiatives are equally successful, however. Those most likely to achieve desired outcomes are based on a clear understanding of targeted health behaviors, and the environmental context in which they occur. Practitioners use strategic planning models to develop and manage these programs, and continually improve them through meaningful evaluation. Health behavior theory can play a critical role throughout the program planning process. What Is Theory? A theory presents a systematic way of understanding events or situations. It is a set of concepts, definitions, and propositions that explain or predict these events or situations by illustrating the relationships between variables. Theories must be applicable to a broad variety of situations. They are, by nature, abstract, and don’t have a specified content or topic area. Like empty coffee cups, theories have shapes and boundaries, but nothing inside. They become useful when filled with practical topics, goals, and problems. • Concepts are the building blocks—the primary elements—of a theory. • Constructs are concepts developed or adopted for use in a particular theory. The key concepts of a given theory are its constructs. • Variables are the operational forms of constructs. They define the way a construct is to be measured in a specific situation. Match variables to constructs when identifying what needs to be assessed during evaluation of a theory- driven program. • Models may draw on a number of theories to help understand a particular problem in a certain setting or context. They are not always as specified as theory. Most health behavior and health promotion theories were adapted from the social and behavioral sciences, but applying them to health issues often requires that one be familiar with epidemiology and the biological sciences. Health behavior and health promotion theories draw upon various disciplines, such as psychology, sociology, anthropology, consumer behavior, and marketing. Many are not highly developed or have not been rigorously tested. Because of this, they often are called conceptual frameworks or theoretical frameworks; here the terms are used interchangeably. How Can Theory Help Plan Effective Programs? Theory gives planners tools for moving beyond intuition to design and evaluate health behavior and health promotion interventions based on understanding of behavior. It helps them to step back and consider the larger picture. Like an artist, a program planner who grounds health interventions in theory creates innovative ways to address specific circumstances. He or she does not depend on a “paint-by­ numbers” approach, re-hashing stale ideas, but uses a palette of behavior theories, skillfully applying them to develop unique, tailored solutions to problems. Using theory as a foundation for program planning and development is consistent with the current emphasis on using evidence- based interventions in public health, behavioral medicine, and medicine. Theory provides a road map for studying problems, developing appropriate interventions, and evaluating their successes. It can inform the planner’s thinking during all of these stages, offering insights that translate into stronger programs. Theory can also help to explain the dynamics of health behaviors, including processes for changing them, and the influences of the many forces that affect health behaviors, including social and physical environments. Theory can also help planners identify the most suitable target audiences, methods for fostering change, and outcomes for evaluation. Researchers and practitioners use theory to investigate answers to the questions of “why,” “what,” and “how” health problems should be addressed. By seeking answers to these questions, they clarify the nature of targeted health behaviors. That is, theory guides the search for reasons why people do or do not engage in certain health behaviors; it helps pinpoint what planners need to know before they develop public health programs; and it suggests how to devise program strategies that reach target audiences and have an impact. Theory also helps to identify which indicators should be monitored and measured during program evaluation. For these reasons, program planning, implementation, and monitoring processes based in theory are more likely to succeed than those developed without the benefit of a theoretical perspective. Explanatory Theory and Change Theory Explanatory theory describes the reasons why a problem exists. It guides the search for factors that contribute to a problem (e.g., a lack of knowledge, self-efficacy, social support, or resources), and can be changed. Examples of explanatory theories include the Health Belief Model, the Theory of Planned Behavior, and the Precaution Adoption Process Model. Change theory guides the development of health interventions. It spells out concepts that can be translated into program messages and strategies, and offers a basis for program evaluation. Change theory helps program planners to be explicit about their assumptions for why a program will work. Examples of change theories include Community Organization and Diffusion of Innovations. Figure 1. illustrates how explanatory theory and change theory can be used to plan and evaluate programs. Fitting Theory to the Field of Practice This monograph includes descriptions and applications of some theories that are central to health behavior and health promotion practice today. No single theory dominates health education and promotion, nor should it; the problems, behaviors, populations, cultures, and contexts of public health practice are broad and varied. Some theories focus on individuals as the unit of change. Others examine change within families, institutions, communities, or cultures. Adequately addressing an issue may require more than one theory, and no one theory is suitable for all cases. 5 PA R T 1 FO U N D A T IO N S O F A P P LY IN G T H E O R Y IN H E A LT H P R O M O T IO N P R A C T IC E Figure 1. Using Explanatory Theory and Change Theory to Plan and Evaluate Programs Problem Behavior or Situation ChangeTheory Which strategies? Which messages? Assumptions about how a program should work Evaluation Planning Explanatory Theory Why? What can be changed? 6 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E Because the social context in which behavior occurs is always evolving, theories that were important in public health education a generation ago may be of limited use today. At the same time, new social science research allows theorists to refine and adapt existing theories. A recent Institute of Medicine report2 observed that several theorists have converged in their views, identifying several variables as central to behavior change. As a result, some constructs, such as self-efficacy, are central to multiple theories. Effective practice depends on using theories and strategies that are appropriate to a situation. One of the greatest challenges for those concerned with behavior change is learning to analyze how well a theory or model “fits” a particular issue. A working knowledge of specific theories, and familiarity with how they have been applied in the past, improves skills in this area. Selecting an appropriate theory or combination of theories helps take into account the multiple factors that influence health behaviors. The practitioner who uses theory develops a nuanced understanding of realistic program outcomes that drives the planning process. Choosing a theory that will bring a useful perspective to the problem at hand does not begin with a theory (e.g., the most familiar theory, the theory mentioned in a recent journal article, etc.). Instead, this process starts with a thorough assessment of the situation: the units of analysis or change, the topic, and the type of behavior to be addressed. Because different theoretical frameworks are appropriate and practical for different situations, selecting a theory that “fits” should be a careful, deliberate process. Start with the steps in the box at the top of the next page. A Good Fit: Characteristics of a Useful Theory A useful theory makes assumptions about a behavior, health problem, target population, or environment that are: • Logical; • Consistent with everyday observations; • Similar to those used in previous successful programs; and • Supported by past research in the same area or related ideas. Using Theory to Address Health Issues in Diverse Populations The U.S. population is growing more culturally and ethnically diverse. An increasing body of research shows health disparities exist among various ethnic and socio-economic groups. These findings highlight the importance of understanding the cultural backgrounds and life experiences of community members, though research has not yet established when and under what circumstances targeted or tailored health communications are more effective than generic ones. (Targeting involves using information about shared characteristics of a population subgroup to create a single intervention approach for that group. In contrast, tailoring is a process that uses an assessment to derive information about one specific person, and then offers change or information strategies for an outcome of interest based on that person’s unique characteristics.)3 Most health behavior theories can be applied to diverse cultural and ethnic groups, but health practitioners must understand the characteristics of target populations (e.g., ethnicity, socioeconomic status, gender, age, and geographical location) to use these theories correctly. There are several reasons why culture and ethnicity are critical to consider when applying theory to a health problem. First, morbidity and mortality rates for different diseases vary by race and ethnicity; second, there are differences in the prevalence of risk behaviors among these groups; and third, the determinants of health behaviors vary across racial and ethnic groups. What People in the Field Say About Theory “Theory is different from most of the tools I use in my work. It’s more abstract, but that can be a plus too. A solid grounding in a handful of theories goes a long way toward helping me think through why I approach a health problem the way I do.” — County Health Educator “I used to think theory was just for students and researchers. But now I have a better grasp of it; I appreciate how practical it can be.” — State Chronic Disease Administrator “By translating concepts from theory into real-world terms, I can get my staff and community volunteers to take a closer look at why we’re conducting programs the way we do, and how they can succeed or fail.” — City Tobacco Control Coordinator “A good grasp of theory is essential for leadership. It gives you a broader way of viewing your work. And it helps create a vision for the future. But, of course, it’s only worthwhile if I can translate it clearly and simply to my co-workers.” — Regional Health Promotion Chief “It’s not as hard as I thought it would be to keep up with current theories. More than ever these days, there are tools and workshops to update us often.” — Patient Education Coordinator 7 PA R T 1 FO U N D A T IO N S O F A P P LY IN G T H E O R Y IN H E A LT H P R O M O T IO N P R A C T IC E Part 2 Theories and Applications 9 PA R T 2 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach Contemporary health promotion involves more than simply educating individuals about healthy practices. It includes efforts to change organizational behavior, as well as the physical and social environment of communities. It is also about developing and advocating for policies that support health, such as economic incentives. Health promotion programs that seek to address health problems across this spectrum employ a range of strategies, and operate on multiple levels. The ecological perspective emphasizes the interaction between, and interdependence of, factors within and across all levels of a health problem. It highlights people’s interactions with their physical and socio­ cultural environments. Two key concepts of the ecological perspective help to identify intervention points for promoting health: first, behavior both affects, and is affected by, multiple levels of influence; second, individual behavior both shapes, and is shaped by, the social environment (reciprocal causation). To explain the first key concept of the ecological perspective, multiple levels of influence, McLeroy and colleagues (1988)4 identified five levels of influence for health- related behaviors and conditions. Defined in Table 1., these levels include: (1) intrapersonal or individual factors; (2) interpersonal factors; (3) institutional or organizational factors; (4) community factors; and (5) public policy factors. 10 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E Figure 2. A Multilevel Approach to Epidemiology Social and Economic Policies Institutions Neighborhoods and Communities Living Conditions Social Relationships Individual Risk Factors Pathophysiological Pathways Individual/Population Health Genetic/Constitutional Factors Envir onm ent Li fe co ur se Source: Smedley BD, Syme SL (eds.), Institute of Medicine. Promoting Health: Strategies from Social and Behavioral Research. Washington, D.C.:, National Academies Press, 2000. Table 1. An Ecological Perspective: Levels of Influence Concept Intrapersonal Level Interpersonal Level Community Level Institutional Factors Community Factors Public Policy Definition Individual characteristics that influence behavior, such as knowledge, attitudes, beliefs, and personality traits Interpersonal processes and primary groups, including family, friends, and peers that provide social identity, support, and role definition Rules, regulations, policies, and informal structures, which may constrain or promote recommended behaviors Social networks and norms, or standards, which exist as formal or informal among individuals, groups, and organizations Local, state, and federal policies and laws that regulate or support healthy actions and practices for disease prevention, early detection, control, and management In practice, addressing the community level requires taking into consideration institutional and public policy factors, as well as social networks and norms. Figure 2. illustrates how different levels of influence combine to affect population health. Each level of influence can affect health behavior. For example, suppose a woman delays getting a recommended mammogram (screening for breast cancer). At the individual level, her inaction may be due to fears of finding out she has cancer. At the interpersonal level, her doctor may neglect to tell her that she should get the test, or she may have friends who say they do not believe it is important to get a mammogram. At the organizational level, it may be hard to schedule an appointment, because there is only a part-time radiologist at the clinic. At the policy level, she may lack insurance coverage, and thus be unable to afford the fee. Thus, the outcome, the woman’s failure to get a mammogram, may result from multiple factors. The second key concept of an ecological perspective, reciprocal causation, suggests that people both influence, and are influenced by, those around them. For example, a man with high cholesterol may find it hard to follow the diet his doctor has prescribed because his company cafeteria doesn’t offer healthy food choices. To comply with his doctor’s instructions, he can try to change the environment by asking the cafeteria manager to add healthy items to the menu, or he can dine elsewhere. If he and enough of his fellow employees decide to find someplace else to eat, the cafeteria may change its menu to maintain lunch business. Thus, the cafeteria environment may compel this man to change his dining habits, but his new habits may ultimately bring about change in the cafeteria as well. 11 PA R T 2 T H E O R IE S A N D A P P LIC A T IO N S 12 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E An ecological perspective shows the advantages of multilevel interventions that combine behavioral and environmental components. For instance, effective tobacco control programs often use multiple strategies to discourage smoking.5 Employee smoking cessation clinics have a stronger impact if the workplace has a no-smoking policy and the city has a clean indoor air ordinance. Adolescents are less likely to begin smoking if their peers disapprove of the habit and laws prohibiting tobacco sales to minors are strictly enforced. Health promotion programs are more effective when planners consider multiple levels of influence on health problems. Theoretical Explanations of Three Levels of … Theory of Reasoned Action, Theory of Planned Behavior, Background The Theory of Reasoned Action is used to explain and predict behavior. Based on attitudes, norms, and intentions Individual motivational factors influence the likelihood of performing a behavior. Developed by Icek Ajzen and Martin Fishbein. (Edberg, 2007) 2 3 3 Behavioral intention is one of the strongest predictors of behavior. Attitude here focuses on towards the behavior not the object (e.g., disease). For example, if the goal is to reduce HIV infections, practitioners should focus on addressing attitudes towards condom use as opposed to teaching HIV pathology. Behavioral Beliefs Beliefs about the likely consequences of the behavior. Produce a favorable or unfavorable attitude towards the behavior. . Example: If I drink too much, then… (Edberg, 2007) 4 Evaluation of Behavioral Beliefs Value attached to outcome or attribute. Produce a favorable or unfavorable attitude towards the behavior. Example: Missing class because I drank too much would be… (Edberg, 2007) 5 Subjective Norms Belief about what is “normal,” and (as defined by the term itself) is subjective (i.e., based on one’s perception) 6 Normative Beliefs Beliefs about whether referent individuals will approve or disapprove of the behavior. Example: If I drank too much, my significant other would… (Edberg, 2007) Motivation to comply refers to the motivation to do what each referent thinks. based on the example above, this could be, “it is important for me to make my significant other happy.” 7 But… Something seemed to be missing. 8 Evolution from TRA to TPB Fishbein & Ajzen realized the TRA does not address external factors. TRA doesn’t account for peoples’ perception of the power or control they have over their behaviors. Perceived Behavioral Control (Edberg, 2007) 9 10 Perceived Behavioral Control Perceived Behavioral Control Control Beliefs Perceived Power Perceived behavioral control: The amount of control over a behavior people perceive that they have 11 Control Beliefs Perceived Behavioral Control Control Beliefs Perceived Power The perceived likelihood of doing the behavior or not. Example: having sex while intoxicated 12 Perceived Power Perceived Behavioral Control Control Beliefs Perceived Power Beliefs regarding the presence of factors that could impact doing the behavior. Example: being intoxicated will make it harder to have sex. 13 General Rule In most cases: the more positive the attitude, the greater the perceived approval (subjective norm), the stronger the perceived control, the more likely the person’s intention is to perform the behavior. 14 15 Critiques The longer the time interval between Behavioral Intention and Behavior, the less likely the behavior will occur. I will work out today vs. I will workout in a couple of months The theory is based on the assumption that human beings are rational and make systematic decisions based on available information. Unconscious motives are not considered. (Glanz, Rimer, & Viswanath, 2008) 16 Critiques Factors such as personality and demographic variables are not taken into consideration. There is much ambiguity regarding how to define Perceived Behavioral Control and this creates measurement problems. Assumption is made that Perceived Behavioral Control predicts actual behavioral control. This may not always be the case. (Glanz, Rimer, & Viswanath, 2008) 17 References Glanz, K., Rimer, B., & Lewis, F. (Eds.). (2002). Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Glanz, K., & Rimer, B. K. (1997). Theory at a glance: a guide for health promotion practice. Bethesda, Md.: U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Cancer Institute. THEORY OF REASONED ACTION Motivation to Comply Behavioral Beliefs Behavioral Intention Behavior Subjective Norm Attitude Toward Behavior Normative Beliefs Evaluations of Behavioral Outcomes THEORY OF REASONED ACTION Behavioral Beliefs Evaluations of Behavioral Outcomes Normative Beliefs Motivation to Comply Attitude Toward Behavior Subjective Norm Behavioral Intention Behavior THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR Behavioral Beliefs Behavior Subjective Norm Attitude Toward Behavior Behavioral Intention Perceived Behavioral Control Perceived Power Control Beliefs Motivation to Comply Normative Beliefs Evaluations of Behavioral Outcomes THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR Behavioral Beliefs Evaluations of Behavioral Outcomes Normative Beliefs Motivation to Comply Attitude Toward Behavior Subjective Norm Behavioral Intention Behavior Control Beliefs Perceived Power Perceived Behavioral Control *Constructs Definition Behavioral intention Perceived likelihood of performing the behavior Attitude Behavioral Belief Belief that behavioral performance is associated with certain attributes or outcomes Evaluation Value attached to a behavioral outcome or attribute Subjective norm Normative Belief Belief about whether each referent approves or disapproves of the behavior Motivation to Comply Motivation to do what each referent thinks Perceived behavioral control Control Belief (TPB) Perceived likelihood of occurrence of each facilitating or constraining condition Perceived power (TPB) Perceived effect of each condition in making behavioral performance difficult or easy *Constructs Definition Behavioral intention Perceived likelihood of performing the behavior Attitude Behavioral Belief Belief that behavioral performance is associated with certain attributes or outcomes Evaluation Value attached to a behavioral out come or attribute Subjective norm Normative Belief Belief about whether each referent approves or disapproves of the behavior Motivation to Comply Motivation to do what each referent thinks Perceived behavioral control Control Belief (TPB) Perceived likelihood of occurrence of each facilitating or constraining condition Perceived power (TPB) Perceived effect of each condition in making behavioral performance difficult or easy Reducing Cyberbullying: A Theory of Reasoned Action-Based Video Prevention Program for College Students Ashley N. Doane1*, Michelle L. Kelley2, and Matthew R. Pearson3 1Psychology Department, Chowan University, Murfreesboro, North Carolina 2Psychology Department, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia 3Center on Alcoholism, Substance Abuse, and Addictions, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Few studies have evaluated the effectiveness of cyberbullying prevention/intervention programs. The goals of the present study were to develop a Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA)-based video program to increase cyberbullying knowledge (1) and empathy toward cyberbullying victims (2), reduce favorable attitudes toward cyberbullying (3), decrease positive injunctive (4) and descriptive norms about cyberbullying (5), and reduce cyberbullying intentions (6) and cyberbullying behavior (7). One hundred sixty-seven college students were randomly assigned to an online video cyberbullying prevention program or an assessment-only control group. Immediately following the program, attitudes and injunctive norms for all four types of cyberbullying behavior (i.e., unwanted contact, malice, deception, and public humiliation), descriptive norms for malice and public humiliation, empathy toward victims of malice and deception, and cyberbullying knowledge significantly improved in the experimental group. At one-month follow-up, malice and public humiliation behavior, favorable attitudes toward unwanted contact, deception, and public humiliation, and injunctive norms for public humiliation were significantly lower in the experimental than the control group. Cyberbullying knowledge was significantly higher in the experimental than the control group. These findings demonstrate a brief cyberbullying video is capable of improving, at one-month follow-up, cyberbullying knowledge, cyberbullying perpetration behavior, and TRA constructs known to predict cyberbullying perpetration. Considering the low cost and ease with which a video-based prevention/intervention program can be delivered, this type of approach should be considered to reduce cyberbullying. Aggr. Behav. 42:136–146, 2016. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Keywords: cyberbullying; prevention; attitudes; norms; college students INTRODUCTION Cyberbullying, defined as “willful and repeated harm inflicted through the use of computers, cell phones, and other electronic devices” (Hinduja & Patchin, 2009, p. 5), has been a frequent topic in the media over the past few years. Reports have shown that youth (e.g., De Nies, Donaldson, & Netter, 2010; Michels, 2008; Smith-Spark & Vandoorne, 2013) and a college student (Friedman, 2010) committed suicide after beingcyberbullied.Despite growing media attention to the issue ofcyberbullying, few cyberbullying prevention/intervention programs have been developed and evaluated (e.g., Lee, Zi-Pei, Svanstr€om, & Dalal, 2013; Menesini, Nocentini, & Palladino, 2012; Williford et al., 2014; W€olfer et al., 2014). Furthermore, these prevention programs have been developed for youth. In the present study, we developed and tested a cyberbullying prevention program for college students based on constructs outlined by the Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA; Ajzen, 1985). Cyberbullying Prevalence Although not as extensive as research on youth (Tokunaga, 2010), a few studies have examined college students’ experiences of cyberbullying. In three studies that assessed cyberbullying experiences during college, approximately 9–11\% of college students reported having been cyberbullied at their university (Kraft & Wang, 2010; Schenk & Fremouw, 2012; Walker, Sockman, & Koehn, 2011). Other studies have found higher rates of cyberbullying victimization among �Correspondence to: Ashley N. Doane, Psychology Department, Chowan University, 1 University Place, Murfreesboro, NC 27855. E-mail: [email protected] Received 16 November 2014; Accepted 30 April 2015 DOI: 10.1002/ab.21610 Published online 9 September 2015 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR Volume 42, pages 136–146 (2016) © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. college students (54\%, Aricak, 2009; 55\%, DilmaSc, 2009; 22\%, MacDonald & Roberts-Pittman, 2010; 18\%, Whittaker & Kowalski, 2015). Perpetration rates have varied from approximately 9–23\% (20\% Aricak, 2009; 23\%, DilmaSc, 2009; 9\%, MacDonald & Roberts- Pittman, 2010; 12\%, Whittaker & Kowalski, 2015). In a recent study of college students, Kokkinos, Antonia- dou, and Markos (2014) identified 11\% of the sample as cyberbullying victims (only), 14\% as cyberbullying perpetrators (only), and 33\% as both cyberbullying victims and perpetrators. However, definitions, regions, modes of communication included (e.g., cell phones, email), specific types of behavior assessed, and assess- ment time frames have varied considerably across these studies. More recently, the Cyberbullying Experiences Survey (CES), a comprehensive measure of cyberbully- ing, was developed. The CES assesses 21 victimization and 20 perpetration behaviors that reflect four distinct types of cyberbullying: deception, malice, public humiliation, and unwanted contact (Doane, Kelley, Chiang, & Padilla, 2013). Using this multidimensional assessment of cyberbullying, approximately 78\% of participants reported experiencing and 53\% reported perpetrating at least one instance of cyberbullying involving deception, 88\% experienced and 78\% perpe- trated cyberbullying involving malice, 73\% experienced and 38\% perpetrated cyberbullying involving public humiliation, and 66\% experienced and 29\% perpetrated cyberbullying involving unwanted contact. Consequences of Cyberbullying Research has shown that cyberbullying victims and perpetrators are at greater risk for mental health and school problems. Cyberbullying victimization in sam- ples of youth is associated with school problems (e.g., suspension, detention, skipping school, and carrying a weapon to school; Ybarra, Diener-West, & Leaf, 2007), emotional problems (e.g., depressive symptoms; Bo- nanno & Hymel, 2013; Perren, Dooley, Shaw, & Cross, 2010; Ybarra, 2004), suicidal ideation (Bonanno & Hymel, 2013; Hinduja & Patchin, 2010), and suicide attempts (Hinduja & Patchin, 2010). In addition, cyberbullying perpetration is also related to depressive symptoms (Bonanno & Hymel, 2013), suicidal ideation (Bonanno & Hymel, 2013; Hinduja & Patchin, 2010), and suicide attempts (Hinduja & Patchin, 2010). In a study of college students, Aricak (2009) found that cyberbullying victims reported higher somatization, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, anxiety, symptoms of obsessive–compulsive disorder, and depression than students not involved in cyberbullying. In addition, those who were both victims and perpetrators of cyberbullying reported higher somatization, anxiety, hostility, and paranoid ideation, as well as more psychotic symptoms than college students not involved in it. Given the frequency of cyberbullying and the negative consequences associated with cyberbullying, low-cost, easily implemented, effective cyberbullying prevention programs are needed for college students. Theory of Reasoned Action The proposed study was guided by the TRA. TRA posits that attitudes toward a particular behavior (i.e., the degree to which it is positively or negatively evaluated), injunctive norms regarding it (i.e., the perception of others’ approval or disapproval of performing it), and descriptive norms (i.e., the perception that others actually engage in it) influence behavioral intentions, which in turn has a direct effect on the behavior itself (Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010). Based on this theory, decreasing positive attitudes toward the behavior and decreasing positive perceived norms about it are expected to decrease intentions to perform the behavior; ultimately, reducing intentions to perform the behavior should reduce the likelihood of it being carried out. Violence prevention researchers have argued for the need to assess attitude change (e.g., Limber, Nation, Tracy, Melton, & Flerx, 2004; Weisz & Black, 2001). Bullies tend to have more positive attitudes towards violence and low empathy toward victims of bullying (Olweus, 1993a). Therefore, Olweus (1993a, 1993b) recommends that bullying interventions focus on changing the attitudes and behavior of bullies and having students empathize with victims. Recent studies on children (Elledge et al., 2013) and college students (Barlett & Gentile, 2012; Boulton, Lloyd, Down, & Marx, 2012; Doane, Pearson, & Kelley, 2014) have found positive relationships between favorable attitudes toward cyberbullying and cyberbullying perpetration. Using the four-factor cyberbullying perpetration scale of the CES, Doane et al. (2014) found that attitudes, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms significantly predicted cyberbullying intentions, and cyberbullying intentions predicted cyberbullying behavior. Although there were some differences between types of cyber- bullying behavior in terms of which specific TRA constructs predicted cyberbullying perpetration, positive attitudes toward cyberbullying was the strongest predictor of cyberbullying intentions, and cyberbullying intentions were substantially related to cyberbullying perpetration. Another study examined the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 2012), which includes the TRA constructs as well as perceived behavioral control, or the degree to which a person believes they are able to perform a particular behavior (Heirman & Walrave, 2012). Likewise, they found that attitudes, subjective norms (i.e., injunctive norms), and perceived behavioral control were significant predictors of Aggr. Behav. Cyberbullying Prevention: Theory of Reasoned Action 137 cyberbullying intentions, which in turn predicted cyberbullying perpetration. Thus, at least two studies provide support that modifying TRA constructs may be an effective means of reducing cyberbullying perpetration. Empathy In addition to TRA constructs, empathy has been associated with cyberbullying. Studies measuring empathy in general (Schultze-Krumbholz & Scheitha- uer, 2009) and empathy specifically related to cyber- bullying (Doane et al., 2014; Steffgen, K€onig, Pfetsch, & Melzer, 2011) found higher levels of empathy to be associated with lower levels of cyberbullying. When examining affective and cognitive empathy separately, relationships with cyberbullying perpetration were inconsistent (Ang & Goh, 2010; Renati, Berrone, & Zanetti, 2012; Topcu & Erdur-Baker, 2012). Doane et al. (2014) found the negative associations between empathy and cyberbullying perpetration to be fully mediated by TRA constructs such that higher empathy was associated with less positive attitudes, lower injunctive norms, and lower descriptive norms for all four types of cyberbully- ing assessed. Cyberbullying Prevention Programs for Youth Several recent multidisciplinary and international efforts have demonstrated the effectiveness of youth cyberbullying prevention programs. These programs vary in length from one-day programs (e.g., W€olfer et al., 2014) to those that take place over the course of the school-year (e.g., ViSC Social Competence Program, Gradinger, Yanagida, Strohmeier, & Spiel, 2015; KiVa Antibullying Program, Williford et al., 2014). Moreover, programs also vary in terms of whether they are taught by trained teachers (Cross, Campbell, Slee, Spears, & Barnes, 2013; Gradinger et al., 2015; W€olfer et al., 2014), involve a combination of peer educators and teacher-led instruction (e.g., Menesini et al., 2012), or are self-guided but allow interaction with other students (e.g., Lee et al., 2013). In some instances, they are delivered in the community (e.g., “Keep it Tame”; Spears & Zeederberg, 2013) or both in school and the community (e.g., Cyber Friendly School Project; see Cross et al., 2013; Campbell, Cross, Spears, & Slee, 2010). Although the results of many of these programs are preliminary, in general, they have shown benefits in terms of improved cyberbullying knowledge and perspective-taking skills and reduced aggressive behavior. Prevention Program Characteristics Although cyberbullying prevention programs have largely relied on in-person instruction and interactions, videos have been used in a variety of prevention programs across other fields, including programs targeting problems such as workplace violence (e.g., Peek-Asa, Casteel, Mineschian, Erickson, & Kraus, 2004), substance abuse or tobacco use (e.g., Ferketich, Kwong, Shek, & Mae, 2007; Ramirez, Gallion, Espinoza, & Chalela, 1999), pathological gambling (e.g., Doiron & Nicki, 2007), and eating disorders (e.g., Heinze, Wertheim, & Kashima, 2000; Withers, Twigg, Wertheim, & Paxton, 2002; Withers & Wertheim, 2004). Prevention programs including videos have been shown to be effective in increasing empathy toward victims of rape (e.g., Foubert & Cowell, 2004; O’Donohue, Yeater, & Fanetti, 2003). Furthermore, participants in Foubert and Cowell’s study rated the video aspect of the program as the most powerful part of the program. In traditional bullying prevention programs, Olweus (1993a) recommends using videos of bullying exam- ples to clarify bullying behavior. The video commonly used in the school-based Olweus Bullying Prevention Program is 11 min in length and consists of four bullying situation vignettes (Olweus, Limber, & Mihalic, 1999). In addition to providing bullying information, the bullying video “elicits emotional, ‘gut feeling’ reactions from the audience” (p. 28). In a study of Italian youth, Baldry and Farrington (2004) evaluated a bullying and victimization intervention program which consisted of three videos, a booklet, role-playing, and discussions. Youth reported signifi- cantly less bullying and victimization at post-inter- vention than pre-intervention. Internet-based prevention programs have been used to target many areas, including smoking (see Walters, Wright, & Shegog, 2006 for a review), HIV (e.g., Bowen, Williams, Daniel, & Clayton, 2008; Roberto et al., 2008), drug abuse (e.g., Schwinn, Schinke, & di Noia, 2010), and depression (e.g., Van Voorhees et al., 2009). Conn (2010) recommends increasing the use of Internet-based health preven- tion programs due to their lower cost, higher consistency, greater accessibility (i.e., both tempo- rally and with physical location), and the ability for program participants to remain anonymous. Extrap- olating from the results of previous studies, a video- based cyberbullying prevention program that con- tains brief informational segments combined with short depictions of common cyberbullying incidents that show victim responses, and peers commenting on the inappropriateness of these actions, may be effective in reducing positive cyberbullying attitudes and behavior. Moreover, the technology currently exists to widely disseminate this type of program at low cost. Aggr. Behav. 138 Doane et al. Present Study Based on the TRA and previous research, the purpose of the present study was to evaluate the success of a video-based cyberbullying prevention program devel- oped by the first author presented in an online format (Doane, 2011). It was predicted that: (1) compared to baseline, positive attitudes toward cyberbullying, pos- itive injunctive and descriptive norms concerning cyberbullying, and intentions to cyberbully would be significantly lower and cyberbullying knowledge and empathy toward cyberbullying victims would be significantly higher in the experimental group immedi- ately after completing the program. In addition, it was hypothesized that: (2) at one-month follow-up, positive attitudes toward cyberbullying, positive injunctive and descriptive norms concerning cyberbullying, intentions to cyberbully, and cyberbullying perpetration would be significantly lower and cyberbullying knowledge and cyberbullying victim empathy would be significantly higher in the experimental than in the control group after controlling for baseline scores, gender, and age. METHOD Participants At a large university in southeastern Virginia, an e- mail invitation to participate in a study on negative experiences via electronic devices was distributed to freshmen (n ¼ 3,187) and sophomores (n ¼ 3,128) who were 18–23 years old. Students were randomly assigned to either the video-based cyberbullying prevention program or no prevention program (assessment-only). Of the 375 students (190 in the experimental group, 185 in the control group) who participated in the initial part of the study (baseline), 167 students (73 in the experimental group, 94 in the control group; 68.7\% females, 31.3\% males; Mage ¼ 19.02, SD ¼ .91) com- pleted both study time points (baseline and one-month follow-up). The majority self-reported their ethnic group as White (62.9\%) or African American (18.0\%). Students were entered into a raffle for a $25 Amazon. com gift certificate for completing the first assessment. For completing the one-month follow-up assessment, participants were entered into a total of 31 raffles (one $50 Amazon.com gift certificate and 30 $15 gift certificates for Amazon.com, Starbucks, Walmart, iTunes, or Subway). For each assessment, participants enrolled in psychology courses were also offered research credit. This study was approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board prior to data collection. Prior to beginning the study, participants read a study description and provided informed consent. More information regarding the 375 students who completed the baseline portion of the present study has been reported elsewhere (Doane et al., 2014). Measures Cyberbullying knowledge. Cyberbullying know- ledge was assessed by a five-item multiple choice quiz based on video content. A sample item is, “Which of the following are individuals who have been cyberbullied at greater risk for?” with the following choices: A. School related problems, B. Alcohol and drug use, C. Attempted suicide, D. All of the above, E. A and C only. CES. The 20-item perpetrator scale of the CES (Doane et al., 2013) was used to measure four types of cyberbullying behavior: malice (six items, as ¼ .89–.90, e.g., “Have you sent a rude message to someone electronically?”), deception (three items, as ¼ .85–.89, e.g., “Have you pretended to be someone else while talking to someone electronically?”), public humilia- tion (three items, as ¼ .76–.93, e.g., “Have you posted an embarrassing picture of someone electronically where other people could see it?”), and unwanted contact (eight items, as ¼ .96, e.g., “Have you sent an unwanted pornographic picture to someone electroni- cally?”). Cyberbullying behavior items are answered on a six-point scale ranging from “Never” (0) to “Everyday/Almost Everyday” (5). For the present study, behavior was assessed for the past month. Convergent validity with Ybarra et al.’s (2007) measure of Internet harassment and the Cyberbullying Assess- ment Instrument (Hinduja & Patchin, 2009) has been established. Empathy toward victims. Based on a study by Endreson and Olweus (2001), to assess empathy toward victims (as ¼ .90–.99), participants reported the degree to which they feel sorry for a person who has experienced each of the 20 cyberbullying behaviors in the CES. For example, “I feel very sorry for a person who has been [teased by others electronically]” was answered on a six-point scale ranging from “Does not apply at all” (0) to “Applies exactly” (5). Four composite scores were formed by averaging across the items in each CES subscale. We have found support for convergent validity of these empathy subscales as they have been shown to be negatively associated with cyberbullying perpetration (Doane et al., 2014). Attitudes, injunctive norms, descriptive norms, and intentions. Based on recommenda- tions by Ajzen (2006), questions were administered that assess attitudes toward cyberbullying, perceived norms concerning cyberbullying, and intentions to engage in cyberbullying behavior. Each group of items comprised all 20 items from the four CES perpetration subscales (Doane et al., 2013). To measure attitudes toward a behavior, Ajzen suggests using adjective scales Aggr. Behav. Cyberbullying Prevention: Theory of Reasoned Action 139 characterizing instrumental (e.g., harmful-beneficial), experiential (e.g., unenjoyable-enjoyable), and overall evaluation (e.g., bad-good). Thus, the item “For me, to [tease someone electronically] in the forthcoming month is” was given for all 20 CES perpetration items and answered on the three six-point scales (0–5) with the anchors listed above. Attitude composite scores (as ¼ .77–.96) were computed by averaging these sets of attitudes items separately for each CES subscale. Higher scores indicated more positive attitudes toward cyberbullying. Injunctive norms (as ¼ .88–.96) were measured by repeating the stem “My peers would ______ of my [teasing someone electronically] in the forthcoming month” for each item with a six-point response scale ranging from “disapprove” (0) to “approve” (5). To report descriptive norms (as ¼ .88–.97), participants selected responses on a six-point scale from “completely false” (0) to “completely true” (5) to the stem “My peers [tease others electronically]” for each item. For both injunctive and descriptive norms, four composite scores were computed by averaging across the 20 CES items. Higher scores indicated more favorable injunctive and descriptive norms regarding cyberbullying. The stem “I intend to [tease someone electronically] within the next month” was answered on a six-point scale ranging from “extremely unlikely” (0) to “extremely likely” (5) for each CES item to measure intentions to engage in cyberbullying (as ¼ .80–.97). The 20 CES items from the four subscales were averaged across to create composite scores. Higher composite scores indicated higher intentions to engage in cyberbullying in the next month. Support for convergent validity of these measures has been established, as attitudes toward cyberbullying, injunctive norms, descriptive norms, and intentions were significantly associated with all four CES perpetration subscales (Doane et al., 2014). Procedure Program development and content. A video- based cyberbullying program (approximately 10 min) was developed for students in the experimental group to view during the online prevention program. During the development phase of the study, a cyberbullying researcher not associated with the project, faculty members, and graduate students reviewed the video content and actor scripts and made suggestions. Once the scripts were finalized, young actors from the participat- ing university were recruited and assigned parts. The first author supervised practices and identified appro- priate set designs. The video-based program was directed, filmed, edited, and the final product developed by the award-winning video production team at the participating university. Given previous support for empathy and TRA constructs as predictors of cyberbullying perpetration (e.g., Doane et al., 2014), our goal was to improve these constructs with the video content. Thus, the cyberbully- ing prevention video alternated between (1) four brief flashes in which actual news stories were summarized about teenagers who were cyberbullied and eventually committed suicide (i.e., to improve victim empathy and attitudes toward cyberbullying); (2) brief attention- grabbing informational slides with voiceovers that presented key information about cyberbullying (e.g., definition of cyberbullying, the different types of cyberbullying, the modes used for cyberbullying, common outcomes associated with cyberbullying, and the prevalence of cyberbullying; i.e., to increase cyberbullying knowledge); and (3) six short, memo- rable, realistic vignettes that consisted of narration and depictions of common cyberbullying events (e.g., receiving mean text messages; i.e., to improve victim empathy and cyberbullying attitudes and norms). The six vignettes are based on actual cyberbullying events and common cyberbullying events identified in previous research. To increase victim empathy and reduce positive cyberbullying attitudes, four vignettes are from the victims’ point-of-view and involve common modes of electronic communication used for cyberbullying (e.g., instant messaging). These scripts illustrate how upset- ting cyberbullying can be. For instance, one video segment shows a female actor sitting at her laptop in her dorm room with multiple instant message windows open with hurtful messages from other people. She then describes how upset she becomes when she receives these messages. To decrease favorable norms, five actors discussed how cyberbullying is unacceptable and not “cool.” The rationale for including young actors discussing the inappropriateness of cyberbullying is that: (1) students may think that their peers believe cyberbullying is unacceptable, and (2) they may perceive that their peers’ frequency of cyberbullying behavior is lower. In other words, the video may decrease injunctive norms and descriptive norms about cyberbullying behavior. For example, one scenario shows a group of students sitting around and talking about their friends’ experiences, and how cyberbullying is “stupid” and “immature.” Pilot study. Prior to testing the program, a pilot study was conducted with 57 college students to determine if the cyberbullying video appeared effective in facilitating the study goals. Results of the pilot test revealed favorable attitudes toward cyberbullying, favorable injunctive and descriptive norms about cyberbullying, and intentions to cyberbully were significantly lower and cyberbullying knowledge and Aggr. Behav. 140 Doane et al. empathy toward victims were significantly higher immediately after viewing the video online than at baseline. Evaluation design. After pilot testing, the pre- vention program was evaluated using two designs. Specifically, the experimental group viewed the cyber- bullying prevention video and participated in a pre-, immediate post-, one-month follow-up design (i.e., completed three assessments), whereas the control group did not view the video (i.e., assessment only) and participated in a pre-, one-month follow-up design (i.e., completed two assessments) at the same time as the pre- and one-month follow-up assessments for the exper- imental group. In the spring of 2011, all freshmen and sophomores enrolled at the participating university who were traditional college age (i.e., 18–23-years-old) were invited to participate via their university e-mail address which included a link to the study. Both the cyberbully- ing prevention group and the control group completed electronic surveys that assessed cyberbullying knowl- edge, cyberbullying attitudes, injunctive and descriptive norms concerning cyberbullying, intentions to engage in cyberbullying, cyberbullying behavior, and empathy toward cyberbullying victims at baseline and one month after baseline. To assess immediate effects of the program, only the experimental group completed the measures of knowledge, cyberbullying attitudes, in- junctive norms, descriptive norms, intentions to cyber- bully, and cyberbullying victim empathy immediately after completing the video-based prevention program. RESULTS Data Analysis Plan Given that most of the outcome variables in the present study were not normally distributed, it is important to use a method that is robust to normality violations (Erceg- Hurn & Mirosevich, 2008); thus, all analyses were conducted with bootstrapping (Efron & Tibshirani, 1993). Rather than relying on a theoretical sampling distribution that is assumed to be normal, bootstrapping creates an empirical sampling distribution by resampling from the sample with replacement. Then, statistical tests are based on these empirically derived sampling distributions. Preliminary Analyses Demographic comparisons were conducted between the study participants and the university population, between students who participated only in the baseline survey and those who completed both time points, and between the experimental and control group. Further- more, participants who completed both assessments were compared to participants who did not complete both assessments on all study variables (knowledge and for all four types of cyberbullying: attitudes, injunctive norms, descriptive norms, intentions, behavior, and empathy toward victims). Compared to the larger population of traditional-aged freshman and sophomore classes at the university, participants in the present study were more likely to … 46 ABSTRACT This study assessed the Integrated Behavioral Model’s (IBM) utility in explaining high-risk drinking among college students. A total of 356 participants completed a four-page questionnaire based on the (IBM) theory and their drinking behavior. The results from a path analysis revealed three significant constructs (p<0.05) which predicted intentions to engage in high-risk drink- ing: experiential attitude (0.34), injunctive norms (0.23), and self-efficacy (-0.28). The IBM explained approximately 45\% and 26\% of variance in intentions and high-risk drinking, respec- tively. Although limited in its use thus far, the IBM shows promise in its application regarding high-risk drinking prevention among college students. Keywords: High-risk drinking, college students, and behavioral science theory Robert E. Braun, Ph.D., MPH, CHES Otterbein University Tavis Glassman, Ph.D., MPH, MCHES, Jiunn-Jye Sheu, Ph.D., MSPH, MCHES, Joseph Dake, Ph.D., MPH, & Tim Jordan, Ph.D., MEd The University of Toledo Faith Yingling, Ph.D., MEd, CHES Bowling Green State University Using the Integrated Behavioral Model to Predict High-Risk Drinking among College Students 47USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING BACKGROUND The drinking behavior college students’ exhibit remains an ongoing public health concern across the nation. High- risk drinking, defined by Johnston and colleagues (2001), as con- suming five or more drinks in one occasion within the previous two weeks, is quite prevalent. With approximately 43\% of the student population engaging in this behavior, for the better part of the last quarter century, the issue appears to be intractable (American College Health Association, 2010; Core Institute at Southern Illinois University, 2011; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2011; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, Seibring, Nelson, & Lee, 2002). College students who drink at these levels are at an increased risk for experiencing a variety of negative health outcomes. Results from the National College Health Assessment II (American College Health Association, 2010) reveal that college students experience the following when they over indulge: regret something they did (35\% of males; 33\% of females), forget where they were or what they did (32\% of males; 28\% of females) and physically injure themselves (18\% of males; 15\% of females). Other consequences suffered from consuming too much alcohol include death, injury, assault, sex- ual abuse, unsafe sex, and family problems (American College Health Association, 2010). The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) provides a number of recommendations on how to remedy this issue including the use of evidence based and theory driven interventions (Presley, Meilman, & Leichliter, 2002). The Integrated Behavioral Model (IBM) represents an emerging the- ory to address health behavior. Much like the Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA) and the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB), the IBM’s predecessors, the IBM posits the intention to perform a behavior as the strongest predictor of behavior; however, this model includes new concepts not utilized within the TPB. The IBM includes three global constructs ― attitude, perceived norm, & personal agency ― with two specific constructs per category. For example, the two constructs that compose attitude are experi- ential and instrumental attitude; within perceived norm includes injunctive and descriptive norms, and personal agency consists of perceived control and self-efficacy. 48 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING The first primary construct within the IBM, attitude, mea- sures the respondent’s feelings toward that behavior. It answers the question, “Does he or she have an unfavorable or favorable beliefs towards performing that behavior” (Montano & Kasprzyk, 2008, pg.78). In the IBM, attitudes are based on experiential and instrumental attitudes. Fishbein (2007) states the emotions asso- ciated with the behavior help to shape experiential attitude. For instance, if an individual had a favorable response in the past to performing a behavior, then he or she is more likely to perform it in the future. Past behavior is an important component of expe- riential attitude and influences future behavior. Instrumental atti- tude, a cognitively based construct, involves the evaluation of the behavior which subsequently influences intentions and future behavior. The second primary construct, perceived norm, is based on social acceptance. This approval can come from a family mem- ber, significant other, or friend (referents). Injunctive norms, in this model, are similar to subjective norms in the TPB. While injunctive norms measure what the beliefs of your referents are, descriptive norms take into account the referent’s behavior. This is important for two reasons. First, it answers the question, “do your referents participate in a particular behavior you are inter- ested in,” and second, “how often do they participate in a par- ticular behavior?” This is critical because the higher the per- ceived prevalence of peers participating in the behavior, the more likely the individual will also engage in the behavior of interest (Montano & Kasprzyk, 2008). The third primary construct, personal agency, consists of per- ceived control and self-efficacy. Perceived control is the percep- tion a respondent has towards the environment around them, and the effect the environment has on their ability to perform a partic- ular behavior; in general, their control of both internal as well as external factors around them. If an individual’s perceived control is high, then he or she believes they can perform the behavior regardless of external influences. Self-efficacy, conversely, is an individual’s belief in their ability or confidence in performing a particular behavior (Montano & Kasprzyk, 2008). Although limited in its use thus far, research has been done with the IBM to assess college students’ use of emergency contra- ception, with the model predicting 50\% in the variance in inten- 49USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING tion (Wohlwend, Glassman, Dake, Jordan, Khuder & Kimmel, 2013). Turchik and Gidycz (2012) conducted a study assessing the sexual risk behaviors among college students and surmised that using additional constructs, accounted for more variance than the TPB alone. Another study was performed comparing the TPB to the IBM; the results indicated that the IBM elicited more variance than the TPB. The authors attribute the difference in the outcomes between the theories to the better fit the IBM con- structs provided (Elliot & Ainsworth, 2012). Thus, a precedent has been set using the IBM to explain various health behaviors with different populations, yet research with this theory is sparse. The purpose of this research is to determine the IBM’s utility in explaining high-risk drinking among college students and to assess which constructs are the most predicative of this behavior. METHODS Participants The sample for this study consisted of college students from a large Midwestern public university. After approval from the University’s Institutional Review Board, the university regis- trar randomly selected 40 classes to administer the survey with. Researchers also employed a shadow sample for this investiga- tion in the event the originally selected course instructors were not able or willing to participate. Out of the 40 randomly selected classes; two classes had duplicate professors, two classes were offered online, three classes were cancelled, and one class was designated for graduate students only. Thus, of the 32 available classes, 16 professors agreed to allow the research team to admin- ister the survey in their classroom. No pattern emerged among respondent versus non-respondent classes in this study. Of the 16 classes included in this study, 356 out of 402 students com- pleted the survey generating a response rate of 89\%. According to Hoyle (1995), in order to show statistical significance for path analyses, a minimal sample size of 250 was required (Hoyle, 1995). A power analysis for this research was conducted using the G*Power 3 software and resulted with a similar minimum sample size projection (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). 50 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING Procedure Researchers employed a cross-sectional research design for this inquiry. After curtailing the list of possible classes to 16, selected dates and times were agreed upon by the researchers and professors to administer the survey. Once in the classroom, the research staff member instructed participants about the informed consent process. Students were told that their voluntary participa- tion in this study would not affect their grade or class standing. After completion of the survey, participants placed the completed form in a brown envelope, walked it to the front of the room, and place it in a locked box. The proctor stressed confidentiality throughout the data collection process. Instrument The current study utilized a customized survey instrument to answer the research questions. Development of this question- naire included a comprehensive literature review focusing on alcohol consumption among college students, and the Integrated Behavioral Model. Additionally, a total of six focus groups (two for abstainers, two for social drinkers, and two for high-risk drinkers) were used to elicit information to help design the sur- vey and related items. The written questionnaire assessed the alcohol related behaviors among college students, comprised of 69 items on four pages. The questions included in this survey consisted of items related to the IBM, drinking habits, and demo- graphics. Questions based on the IBM were developed using either a 7-point semantic differential or Likert-type scale. All of these scales were developed using the suggestions by Montano and Kasprzyk (2008). Three types of validity were assessed in this study: face, con- tent, and construct validity. Five experts reviewed the question- naire for face validity (i.e., formatting, readability and general organization of the instrument) and content validity of items. The experts included two Alcohol, Tobacco, and Other Drugs (ATOD) practitioners, two ATOD researchers, and one psychometric expert. A Principle Components Analysis (PCA) was conducted using Varimax Rotation to assess construct validity. Consistent with the IBM, the results of the PCA yielded eight constructs or themes. 51USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING The results from the test/retest reliability were all significant at p<0.01 level with Pearson coefficient values listed as fol- lows: instrumental attitude (r=0.87), injunctive norms (r=0.79), self-efficacy (r=0.78), descriptive norms (r=0.76), experiential attitude (r=0.73), perceived control (r=0.62), and behavioral intention (r=0.60). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for each of the sub-constructs also demonstrated the instrument’s reliability: experiential attitude (α=0.96), behavioral intention (α=0.92), per- ceived control (α=0.91), instrumental attitude (α=0.89), self-effi- cacy (α=0.86), injunctive norms (α=0.84), and descriptive norms (α=0.82). More than two-thirds of the survey was dedicated for the measurement of the IBM constructs. To assess the theory, the researchers measured each construct. The measurement of instru- mental attitudes and experiential attitudes constructs included five items each. The response style for each item used a semantic differential style response with 7 potential responses anchored by two polar opposite anchor descriptors. For instrumental attitudes, descriptor examples include “bad-good” and “risky-not risky.” Example anchors for experiential attitudes included “embarrass- ing-not embarrassing” and “not fun-fun.” The measurement for injunctive norms, descriptive norms, and behavioral intention included four items each. The response style for each item used a Likert-type scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” The perceived control construct included five items, coded on a scale, yielding potential scores ranging from one through seven. The response style for each item used a Likert-type scale ranging from “totally not under my control” to “totally under my control.” Finally, the measurement of self-efficacy included five items with a response options scale ranging from “very difficult” to “very easy” on a seven-point scale. Data Analysis Data analysis for this study utilized SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) version 17. Statistical analyses for this investigation assumed a Type I error of 0.05. Descriptive sta- tistics, including frequencies, means, proportions, percentages, and standard deviations, were calculated to describe the sam- ple. Nonparametric tests such as the Chi-square was used for this study due to the non-normal distribution of the data obtained. 52 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING The path analysis was conducted using, EQS v6.1, a structural equation modeling software. RESULTS As Table 1 illustrates, participants in this study included 171 males (48.2\%) and 184 females (51.8\%). Approximately 75\% of the respondents identified themselves as Caucasian (74.7\%; n=263), followed by African-American (10.5\%; n=37), Asian or Pacific Islander (6.3\%; n=22), Hispanics (3.7\%, n=13), and oth- ers (4.8\%; n=17). The mean age of the participants was 23.4 years (SD=5.9 years), with the minimum and maximum ages ranging from 19 and 60, respectively. Third-year undergraduate students made up the largest proportion of respondents (41.5\%; n=146) followed by 2nd year (21.9\%; n=77), 4th year (20.5\%; n=72), 5th year or greater (11.1\%; n=39), and first year (5.1\%; n=18). Finally, 37\% of the respondents reported high-risk drinking the last time they partied/socialized. TABLE 1 Participant Demographics Characteristic Frequency Percent SD Gender Male 171 48.2 Female 184 51.8 Age 23±6 Year in School 1st Year 18 5.1 2nd Year 77 21.9 3rd Year 146 41.5 4th Year 72 20.5 5th Year or greater 39 11.1 Ethnicity African American (Black) 37 10.5 Asian or Pacific Islander 22 6.3 Caucasian (White) 263 74.7 Hispanic (Latino) 13 3.7 Others 17 4.8 53USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING Means, Standard Deviations, and zero-ordered correlations were conducted to describe participants’ perceptions and to deter- mine the association among the IBM constructs (Table 2). The mean values for experiential attitude and instrumental attitude indicate that student perceptions were neutral concerning high- risk drinking affect and outcome beliefs. Conversely, a mean of 2.38 for injunctive norm demonstrated their referent’s disapproval to perform this behavior while a mean of 4.43 among descriptive norm indicated uncertainty regarding their referents high-risk drinking behaviors. Perceived control and self-efficacy elicited high mean values which signify confidence and strong personal control concerning their intentions to engage in high-risk drink- ing. The results from Table 2 showed that with the exception of descriptive norms and perceived control, all constructs were correlated with one another. Conducting a correlation matrix is a prerequisite step (assess the data) to performing a path analy- sis. Overall, the correlation values indicate statistically signifi- cant relationships between the variables, but were not highly cor- Greek Status No 306 86.7 Yes 47 13.3 Member of an NCAA Team No 345 98.0 Yes 7 2.0 Participation in Intramural/Club No 272 77.3 Yes 80 22.7 Hours spent drinking 4±2.4 Those who high-risk drank the last time they partied or socialized No 224 63.3 Yes 130 36.7 # of alcoholic beverages consumed 5±5.4 # of drinks consumed to become drunk 5±6 Enrollment Status Part-time 39 11.1 Full-time 311 88.9 54 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING related. The IBM variables are independent from one another, thus limiting concerns of multicolinearity. Figure 1 depicts the results from the path analysis illustrating the relationships among the IBM constructs. Using the maximum likelihood estimation, the model accounted for 45\% of the vari- TA B L E 2 D es cr ip tiv e St at is tic s an d C or re la tio ns fo r th e IB M a ss oc ia te d w ith H ig h- R is k D ri nk in g M ea n SD E A IA IN D N PC SE B I E A 3. 82 1. 75 - .8 8* ** .6 4* ** .5 8* ** - .1 5* * -.3 5* ** .6 2* ** IA 3. 45 1. 42 - 5 7* ** .4 8* ** -.1 4* -.2 8* ** .5 8* ** IN 2. 38 1. 39 - .5 4* ** - .1 6* * -.3 5* ** .5 1* ** D N 4. 43 1. 58 - -. 10 -.3 0* ** .3 8* ** PC 6. 44 1. 14 - .5 2* ** -.2 3* ** SE 6. 16 1. 16 - -.4 0* ** B I 3. 25 2. 10 - * p< 0. 05 ; * * p< 0. 01 ; * ** p< 0. 00 1. N ot e: E A = e xp er ie nt ia l a tti tu de , I A = in st ru m en ta l a tti tu de , I N = in ju nc tiv e no rm s D N = d es cr ip tiv e no rm s, P C = p er ce iv ed c on tr ol , S E = s el f- ef fic ac y, a nd B I = b eh av io ra l i nt en tio ns 55USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING ance (R2) in the intention to high risk drinking. Three constructs, experiential attitude, injunctive norm, and self-efficacy were statistically significant (p<0.05) with path coefficients of 0.34, 0.23, and -0.28 respectively. Results also indicated the model predicted 26\% of the variance in high-risk drinking. Overall, the model yielded acceptable model fit indices, as demonstrated by the Joreskog-Sorbom’s Goodness of Fit (GFI) Index (.97), Comparative Fit (CFI) Index (.90), and Root Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) of 0.15 and its 90\% confidence interval of 0.11- 0.19. Although model fit indices are descriptive in nature with- out a clear cut point for significance level, a CFI value greater than or equal to 0.95 and a RMSEA value less than or equal to 0.05 are suggested as adequate fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1992; Hu & Bentler, 1999). The model fit indices suggest the data fit at acceptable ranges. Another path model was generated to assess the three primary constructs associated with the IBM, i.e. attitude (ATT), perceived norm (PN), and personal agency (PA), in predicting behavioral Figure 1: Path Analysis of High-Risk Drinking using the IBM ATTITUDE Experiential Attitude PERSONAL AGENCY Instrumental Attitude PERCEIVED NORM Injunctive Norms Perceived Control Descriptive Norms Behavioral Intentions High-Risk Drinking R2=.45 R2=.26 Self-Efficacy 0.34* 0.17 0.23* -0.04 -0.06 -0.28* 0.03* Note: *equals statistically significant at p<0.05 56 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING intentions to engage in high-risk drinking. Each of the primary constructs was created by combining the respective sub-con- structs. All three constructs, ATT, PN, and PA, exhibited statisti- cal significance (p<0.05) with path coefficients of 0.27, 0.10, and -0.17, and accounted for 44\% of the variance in behavioral inten- tion. Overall, the model showed an acceptable model fit indices, as exhibited by the Joreskog-Sorbom’s GFI Index (.95), Joreskog- Sorbom’s AGFI Comparative Fit Index (.9175), and RMSEA of 0.19 with a 90\% C.I. of 0.14-0.25 (see Table 3). Although the goodness of fit estimates were within acceptable range, the Chi- square values (statistically significant) associated with each path analysis did not fall within acceptable limits which is not uncom- mon with large sample sizes (Kline, 1998). DISCUSSION Alcohol abuse continues to compromise academic perfor- mance and student health at colleges and universities across the country (Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, 2009). Utilizing theory to design interventions to help address this issue represents a funda- mental approach in attempting to change this entrenched behavior (NIAAA, 2010). This study used the IBM, the latest iteration of the Theory of Reasoned Action/Theory of Planned Behavior in an attempt to better understand the high-risk drinking patterns of college students. Each of the three primary constructs, i.e. atti- tude (ATT), perceived norm (PN), and personal agency (PA), and their sub-constructs were assessed to predict behavioral intention and high-risk drinking. A path analysis was conducted to determine the direction and significance of the IBM constructs to predict behavioral intentions and high risk drinking. The results revealed the IBM explained approximately 44\% and 26\% of variance in intentions and high- risk drinking, respectively. These findings are consistent with the meta-analytic review published by Armitage and Conner (2001), which reported on average, for any behavior, the TPB explained 39\% of the variance in behavioral intention and 27\% in behavior. Results from the path analysis also revealed that experiential attitude was the strongest positive predictor of intention to engage in high-risk drinking, which reflects trends in this area of research (Elliot & Ainsworth, 2012). Favorable attitudes towards alcohol consumption were indicative of high-risk drinking. Attitudes are 57USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING based in part, by the previous experiences people have. The more positive the experience, the more likely they are to perform the behavior again. Challenging alcohol expectancies and lessening student’s beliefs about benefits of high-risk drinking remains an intervention focal point based on this and other studies (Ham, 2009). Consistent with the literature, injunctive norms yielded a sta- tistically significant, albeit, moderate path coefficient value of 0.23 (Ham, 2009; McMillan & Conner, 2003). Injunctive norms depict the referent’s approval or disapproval in performing a particular behavior. Perhaps, the relatively low value was due to the fact that some respondents are more motivated by certain referents than others. For example, college students may seek approval from their peers but not their parents, thus diminishing the predictive value of this construct. These results corroborate similar outcomes in which the subjective norms construct is the weakest predictor within the TPB (Armitage & Conner, 2001). Nevertheless, in this study and others, injunctive norms, to some degree, influence the drinking patterns among college students (Park, Klein, Smith, & Martell, 2009). In this study, self-efficacy yielded a statistically significant, moderate path coefficient of -0.28. Self-efficacy is an internal belief a person has to perform a particular behavior within a spe- cific context. In this study, results yielded a negative path coeffi- cient, which indicate an inverse relationship between self-efficacy and behavioral intention. Thus, as self-efficacy became stronger, intention to high-risk drink lessened, and vice versa. For exam- ple, if participants believed they had the confidence to refuse alcohol consumption then they tended not to engage in high-risk drinking. Collins and Carey (2007) also found a negative link between self-efficacy and intention. This finding indicates that prevention efforts should target college student’s self-efficacy, perhaps focusing on peer refusal skills. The behavioral intention construct predicted 26\% of the vari- ance in high-risk drinking, which is similar to the results by other researchers (Armitage & Conner, 2001). However, the complex- ities surrounding intentions and actual behavior merit further examination, as intentions are not always predictive of behav- ior. For example, unique circumstances may result in different intentions or the need for individuals to change their original 58 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING intention, some of which may not be captured with traditional survey data. How do intentions change when alcohol is free or when somebody is pursuing a “significant other” are just a cou- ple of examples, which could influence the findings. In general, the more complicated the behavior or social dynamics the more challenging it is to assess intentions. The time between intentions and behavior is yet another variable to consider with this type of research. Nevertheless, behavioral intention within the TRA/ TPB/IBM consistently predicts drinking behavior within the col- lege population (Collins & Carey, 2007; O’Callaghan, Chant, Callan, & Baglioni, 1997). Somewhat unexpectedly, neither instrumental attitude, descriptive norms nor perceived control predicted intentions to engage in high-risk drinking with statistical significance. Thus, to examine the efficacy of the IBM further a path analysis was per- formed using exclusively the three primary constructs within the IBM. The results showed that each of the primary constructs were statistically significant, with the model explaining approximately 44\% of the intention to engage in high-risk drinking. Similar, to the first path analysis attitude was the strongest predictor followed by personal agency and then perceived norms. The findings from this study indicate that the IBM provides utility in explaining high-risk drinking among college students. More specifically, researchers and practitioners should focus on experiential attitude, injunctive norms, and self-efficacy in design- ing interventions with this population and behavior. The preci- sion the IBM provides in identifying which specific constructs to address when combating high-risk drinking demonstrates its use- fulness beyond the theory’s predecessors, the Theory of Reasoned Action and Theory of Planned Behavior. Limitations Several limitations exist within the current study. As with most surveys, the use of self-reported data merit concern, particularly recall bias (Portney & Watkins, 2000). Indeed, respondents may not remember the number of alcoholic beverages they consumed the last time they partied and/or socialized, or may not remember suffering a consequence due to their drinking behavior. The sam- ple was obtained from 16 of the 32 randomly selected classes — represents another concern — response bias. However, the focus 59USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING of this study was on the student response rate, not the instructor. The participants in the sample closely matched the overall student population, with the exception of the small number of first year students. This may have been due to the time of the year when the study was conducted, students matriculating through their respec- tive programs, and students entering the university with college credits obtained from high school. Regardless, the purpose of this study was not to generalize data, per se, but to assess a theory, which the sample provided an adequate means to accomplish this objective. In addition, a cross-sectional research design was employed for this study, thus causal inferences cannot be made. For example, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors do not always change concurrently, because these variables are constantly fluc- tuating, this may have possibly affected the results. Also, the items used to assess instrumental attitude, descriptive norm, and perceived control might not have accurately assessed these con- structs, which may explain the insignificant values yielded from this study. Finally, a theory cannot be proven or disproven with one study, thus additional studies need to be conducted to further assess the efficacy of the IBM. Indeed, a number of recommendations for future research using the IBM emerged from this investigation. First, to more accurately assess the utility of the IBM a time-series research design is suggested. The IBM posits that the intention to perform a behavior is the strongest predictor of behavioral performance. Assessing a respondent’s intention to perform a behavior at time one and measuring how much they performed the behavior at time two warrants additional inquiry. Further, in order to assess con- struct validity more effectively, prospective research needs to be conducted with the IBM. Additional assessments should include other high-risk behaviors, such as marijuana use, cigarette smok- ing, or prescription drug abuse. There are many applications for this model; expanding it to other populations such as minorities, athletes, or Greek social fraternities or sororities represents addi- tional possibilities. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Robert E. Braun PhD, MPH, CHES, Assistant Professor, Otterbein University, Department of Health and Sport Sciences, 160 Center Street, Rike 226, Westerville, OH 43081; Phone: (614) 823-3535; Fax: (614) 823-1965; Email: [email protected] edu. 60 USING IBM TO PREDICT HIGH-RISK DRINKING … Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wssr20 Journal of Social Service Research ISSN: 0148-8376 (Print) 1540-7314 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wssr20 Health Education Intervention on HIV/AIDS Prevention Behaviors among Health Volunteers in Healthcare Centers: An Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior Hadi Alizadeh Siuki, Nooshin Peyman, Mohammad Vahedian-Shahroodi, Mahdi Gholian-Aval & Hadi Tehrani To cite this article: Hadi Alizadeh Siuki, Nooshin Peyman, Mohammad Vahedian-Shahroodi, Mahdi Gholian-Aval & Hadi Tehrani (2019) Health Education Intervention on HIV/AIDS Prevention Behaviors among Health Volunteers in Healthcare Centers: An Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior, Journal of Social Service Research, 45:4, 582-588, DOI: 10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 Published online: 04 Oct 2018. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 730 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 19 View citing articles https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wssr20 https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wssr20 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=wssr20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=wssr20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-04 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-10-04 https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177#tabModule https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177#tabModule Health Education Intervention on HIV/AIDS Prevention Behaviors among Health Volunteers in Healthcare Centers: An Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior Hadi Alizadeh Siukia , Nooshin Peymanb , Mohammad Vahedian-Shahroodib , Mahdi Gholian-Avalb, and Hadi Tehranib aDepartment of Health Education and Health Promotion, School of Health, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran; bDepartment of Health Education and Health Promotion, Social Determinants of Health Research Center, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Mashhad, Iran ABSTRACT Present study aims to investigate the impact of health education intervention on improving HIV/AIDS preventive behaviors among Health volunteers in Torbat-e Heydarieh Iran. 120 Health volunteers were involved in this quasi-experimental study (60 participants in inter- vention group and 60 participants in control group). Data collection tool was a question- naire based on theory of planned behavior along with demographic questions which was applied before and 2 month after intervention. Data analysis was conducted using SPSS16 with descriptive analysis, and analytical tests (independent t-test, correlation coefficient) were conducted at 5\% significance level. The findings of the study showed no significant difference between two groups in terms of mean scores of knowledge, attitude, perform- ance, behavioral intention, and behavioral control before intervention (p > 0.05). But a sig- nificant difference was observed between two groups after the health education intervention (p < 0.05). Health education intervention based on theory of planned behavior, regarding AIDS disease preventive behavior, significantly affects Health volunteers. Therefore, according to behavior theory, such educations lead to positive changes in AIDS preventive behaviors. KEYWORDS Health behavior; education; health volunteers; AIDS Introduction The main cause of AIDS (acquired immune defi- ciency syndrome) is a retrovirus that when enters into body attacks to the T Helper cells which play a key role in defense system of the body, disables immune system cells, and increasingly disrupts body defense against diseases. Incubation period of the disease is long and may take 5–10 years or more, or HIV infection may remain a lifelong (Morlat et al., 2014). AIDS is a health, social, and psychological crisis that not only affects adults but also children and adolescents (Kelly & Lawrence, 2013), so that it can be said that currently AIDS is the problem of young peo- ple that 85 percent of them live in developing countries (Atwine, Cantor-Graae, & Bajunirwe, 2005). On the other hand, 50 percent of new cases infected with the virus happen in people aged 10–24 years old (Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS, & World Health Organization, 2007). According to WHO 7.000 individuals per day, i.e., 5 young people per minute get infected with this virus (UNICEF, Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS, & World Health Organization, 2002). Different statistics show that approximately 39.5 million people are infected with this disease around the world and every year 2 million people die due to this disease (Heald, 2006). From 1999 to 2009, a growth of 27\% has happened in the number of peo- ple with HIV infection (Mee et al., 2014). According to the latest statistics on HIV/ADIS in the Islamic Republic of Iran, published by the Ministry of Health, Treatment and Medical Education, from the beginning of the epidemic until April 2011, 22,727 individuals infected with CONTACT Hadi Tehrani [email protected] Department of Health Education and Health Promotion, Social Determinants of Health Research Center, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, 91778-99191 Mashhad, Iran. � 2018 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC JOURNAL OF SOCIAL SERVICE RESEARCH 2019, VOL. 45, NO. 4, 582–588 https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177&domain=pdf https://ORCID.org/0000-0003-2053-3229 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6218-4787 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5402-1646 http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8747-8717 https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2018.1481177 http://www.tandfonline.com HIV are identified in the country, where 91.7 per- cent of them are men and 8.3 percent are women (Fallahzadeh, Morowatisharifabad, & Ehrampoosh, 2009). In Torbat-e Heydarieh, 4 cases are reported that 1 of them is male and three others are female. The first case was reported in 2010 and one of the cases has led to death (Fallahzadeh et al., 2009). Currently, a large num- ber of health volunteers around the Iran’s country cooperate with healthcare centers and after pass- ing training courses, convey the acquired infor- mation to the covered household and attempt to realize healthcare objectives as well as providing other services such as case-finding, health educa- tion, patient care, raising awareness, and provid- ing public health services (Eslami, Marzban, & Mazloomy, 2016). Theory of planned behavior is one of the healthcare theories which is considered the best and most perfect theory to study behavior (Vafaeenajar et al., 2015). This theory was presented in 1988 by Ajzen and through develop- ment of reasoned action theory (Ajzen & Madden, 1986). The structures of the model include: behavior_intention of behavior_attitude toward behavior_control of perceived behavior_ subjective norm (Gebreeyesus Hadera, Boer, & Kuiper, 2007). In present study, using theory of planned behavior as theoretical framework of the study, a training program will be developed and implemented to improve the awareness and attitude of health contact persons toward AIDS and its preventive methods. According to some studies, training based on theory of planned behavior helps to the creation of the required skills to postpone the suggestions of high risk behaviors regarding AIDS and leads to training and attitude improvement through peers and consequently leads to attitudes and awareness reinforcement (Armitage & Conner, 2001; Eggers, Taylor, Sathiparsad, Bos, & de Vries, 2015; Tyson, Covey, & Rosenthal, 2014). Today, AIDS is an obstacle to economic, social, and cultural development, so that it has turned to a compli- cated social and economic emergency. Given eco- nomic and social loses resulted from expansion of this disease, it is considered the most import- ant challenge against development (Duflo, Dupas, & Kremer, 2015). Informing the community to a large extent can decrease the number of people affected with AIDS (Shojaeizadeh, Taheri, & Tehrani, 2012). To that end, using theory of planned behavior, the present study aims to improve HIV/AIDS preventing behaviors among Health volunteers of Torbat-e Heydarieh in Iran. Method Study Design and Study Target Present study is a quasi-experimental study (60 participants in intervention group and 60 partici- pants in control group) which was conducted in 2016 in healthcare centers. The study population included health volunteers. Consideration a loss rate of 10\%, the total sample size was calculated 120 participants. The participants were in the age range of 18–50 years. The inclusion criteria were to have at least middle school education and not moving resident place since the beginning of the study. The participants were entered into study through cluster sampling method. The participants were permitted to leave the study at any stage; if they were not interested to con- tinue and they were excluded if didn’t take part in two or more training sessions. To that end, after coordination with University authorities the author referred to health deputy of the Torbat-e Heydarieh City and obtained the list of healthcare centers. Then two centers were selected randomly as intervention centers and two other centers as control centers. It should be noted that interven- tion centers had similar conditions in terms of cultural and geographical characteristics. Before filling the questionnaires regarding research objectives, the manner of filling the questionnaire and other required explanations were presented and the questionnaires were completed at the presence of the author. After conducting the initial test, the data were extracted and analyzed. Then, emphasizing theoretical structures of planned behavior (attitude toward behavior, behavior or performance, behavior intention, subjective norms, perceived behavior control) and awareness and performance regarding AIDS preventive behaviors, the author embarked for developing training materials and implement- ing intervention. Training materials regarding JOURNAL OF SOCIAL SERVICE RESEARCH 583 HIV/AIDS were formulated according to the latest epidemiological information of Iran and other countries of the world, in accordance with social and cultural conditions, and were presented by author to 30 people groups through two workshops using PowerPoint slides, pam- phlets, and leaflets. One week after first workshop, the second workshop was held and besides reviewing the headlines of educational materials, the ground was prepared for group discussion and answering the questions. Two month after completion of educational interventions, using a written ques- tionnaire similar to the initial test, the second test was conducted simultaneously for interven- tion and control groups. Measures Data collection tool was a two-part questionnaire. The first part included demographic questions and second part included awareness questions (9 items), attitude toward behavior (10 items), behavior or performance (6 items), behavior intention (4 items), subjective norms (4 items), and Perceived behavioral control (6 items). The content validity was obtained by 7 professors and reliability was calculated 82.0 through Cronbach’s alpha. Scoring the questionnaire was in this way that regarding awareness questions yes answers received two points and I don’t know answers received one point. Scoring attitude, subjective norms, and Perceived behavioral control was in this way that yes answers received two points, no answers received zero points, I have no idea answers received 2 negative points and I disagree answers received one point. Regarding reverse questions in the part of attitude, subjective norms, and Perceived behavioral control, scoring was in this way that I agree answers received one point, I have no idea answers received two nega- tive points, and I’m disagree answers received 3 points. In the part of behavioral questions, yes answers received 1 point, and no answers received any points and scoring was reverse regarding the question with negative load. Regarding behavioral intention, the questions always take 3 or 2 points and never take one point. Statistical Analyses After collecting the questionnaires, data analysis was conducted using SPSS version 16 and descriptive analysis (frequency, percentage, mean, and standard deviation), and analytical tests (independent t-test, paired t-test, correlation coef- ficient) were conducted in a significance level of 0.05. Ethics Approvals Ethical approval was gained from the Torbat-e Heydarieh University of Medical Sciences Ethics Committee. The health volunteers were explained the purpose of the study and consent letter was taken. Written informed consents were obtained from all the study participation. Moreover, they were assured that all the information would be kept confidential and would not be revealed unless for research purposes and in an anonym- ous form. Participants were allowed to decline participation at any stage of research. As part of the consenting process, participants were reminded that their participation in this research was confidential and that no personal identifiable information will be stored with the data. Results In the study, the mean age and standard deviation of the intervention and control groups were 33.13 ±7.10 and 32.68 ±6.62years, respectively. According to the results, two groups are homoge- neous in terms of demographic variables (age, edu- cation, marital status, and occupation) (Table 1). According to independent t-test, no significant difference was observed between two groups before intervention in terms of all structures of theory of planned behavior, but after intervention the scores increased and the differences were stat- istically significant (Table 2). Paired t statistical test shows that the mean of awareness, perform- ance, behavioral intention, subjective norms, and the control of intervention and control groups behaviors before training had no significant dif- ference (p > 0.05) but after training and holding education course, the mean score of above men- tioned variables showed a significant difference in intervention and control groups (p < 0.05). It can 584 H. A. SIUKI ET AL. be said that training intervention significantly affected all structures of planned behaviors in health contact persons (Table 2). Also, a significant correlation was observed regarding the components of theory of planned behavior between health contact persons in inter- vention and control groups after training inter- vention (awareness and behavioral intention (p ¼ 0.01), attitude and performance (p ¼ 0.08) and subjective norms (p ¼ 0.003), performance and subjective norms (p ¼ 0.003), behavioral intention and subjective norms (p ¼ 0.002), sub- jective norms and behavioral control (p ¼ 0.01)) (Table 3). Discussion The significant changes in awareness scores in intervention group, compared to control group, have been due to direct impact of training inter- vention on health contact persons and this training has increased awareness. This increase is in line with many other related studies. A study by Alizadeh Siuki et al. (2013) and Pakpour Hajiagha, Mohammadi Zeidi, and Mohammadi Zeidi (2012) showed an increase in awareness score which is in line with the findings of present study. Since the first step to change behavior is increasing awareness, different ways exist to that end, such as training through self-learning voice messages, face to face training, group discussion, and movies (Reeves, Perrier, Goldman, Freeth, & Zwarenstein, 2013). In present study, PowerPoint slides, pamphlets, and leaflets were used to increase awareness in health contact persons and were identified as useful tools. In present study, a significant difference was observed between attitude scores before and after intervention which indicates substantial changes in the attitudes of health contact persons. It seems that the increase of attitude score among health surveyed health contact persons is due to their awareness increase and positive impact of training interventions. A study by Mohammadi Zeidi, Pakpour Hajiagha, and Mohammadi Zeidi, (2013) shows also an increase in attitude com- pared to behavior after intervention which is con- sistent with the findings of present study. The mean score of behavior performance before and after training showed a significant dif- ference in intervention group. This significant increase in intervention group was an expected finding and shows positive impact of presented training course regarding AIDS preventive Table 2. Comparison between changes in the behavior and constructs of TPB before and after the intervention. Post-intervention Pre-intervention p Value�Mean ± SD Mean ± SD Knowledge Control group 12/11 ± 2/40 12/15 ± 2/42 0/673 Experimental group 11/03 ± 2/82 12/48 ± 1/85 0/0001 p value�� 0/34 0/001 Attitude toward behavior Control group 24/68 ± 2/24 2/26 ± 7340 0/49 Experimental group 22/26 ± 2/19 25/36 ± 3/13 0/0001 p value 0/24 0/002 Behavior Control group 1/10 ± 2/58 1/11 ± 2/61 0/159 Experimental group 0/98 ± 2/98 1/49 ± 3/83 0/001 p value 0/15 0/0001 Subjective norm Control group 1/31 ± 11 1/49 ± 11/15 0/172 Experimental group 1/09 ± 11/46 1/82 ± 12/20 0/003 p value 0/922 0/002 Perceived behavioral control Control group 1/07 ± 14/21 1/09 ± 14/30 0/199 Experimental group 0/46 ± 14/13 0/68 ± 14/33 0/013 p value 0/170 0/003 Behavioral Intention Control group 0/68 ± 9/55 0/71 ± 9/70 0/162 Experimental group 2/09 ± 9/30 1/30 ± 11/13 0/001 p value 0/001 0/392 ��Independent t-test; �Paired t-test. Table 1. Mean and standard deviations of demographic varia- bles in the intervention and control groups. Control group Intervention group Number (\%) Number (\%) Level of education Diploma 48 (80) 49 (81/7) Associate Degree 9 (15) 7 (11/6) Bachelor Degree 3 (5) 4 (6/6) Total 60 (100) 60 (100) Job Housewife 41 (68/3) 42 (70) Employee 9 (15) 8 (13/3) Student 2 (3/4) 3 (5) Others 8 (13/3) 7 (11/7) Total 60 (100) 60 (100) Marital status Single 10 (16/7) 9 (15) Married 50 (83/3) 51 (85) Total 60 (100) 60 (100) JOURNAL OF SOCIAL SERVICE RESEARCH 585 behaviors on improvement of the above men- tioned concept. Also, as the results show, the intervention has been successful in present study. This finding is consistent with the findings of a related study by Medley, Kennedy, O’Reilly, and Sweat (2009). Behavior usually happens following intention and without intention no behavior happens. In present study, the mean score of behavioral intention was good in both groups and a significant increase was observed in behavioral intention of the intervention group after intervention. Highness of behavioral intention in both groups before intervention has positive impact on increase in AIDS protective behaviors among participants. In a related study by Pakpour Hajiagha et al. (2012) Training inter- vention led to improving the skills of rejecting and postponing high-risk behaviors. The increase in behavioral intention mean score probably has been because of appropriate training of health contact persons through lectures and group discussions. The mean score of subjective norms in inter- vention group had a significant increase, while this difference wasn’t significant in control group. Subjective norms occur due to the performance of some believes that particular people may con- firm or not confirm conducting them. Present study shows that community members and the households of health contact persons have high expectations of health contact persons in terms of adopting AIDS protective behaviors. The findings are consistent with a related study by Meister, Grugel, & Meis (2014), where using theory of planned behavior they showed the significant impact of training on attitude change in interven- tion group. But as it was mentioned before, no significant increase was observed regarding the behavior performance of health contact persons in control group. The results of control group suggest that so long as there is no motivational initial condition and training plans are not devel- oped regarding AIDS protective behaviors, no significant change happens in behaviors. The results of the study regarding the impact of training intervention show an increase in perceived behavior control variable in intervention group after training, while the mean score of behavioral control in control group shows no significant difference. This finding is consistent with findings of a related study by Sadeghi & Khanjani (2015). In present study, the author faced some limita- tions, such as: difficulties in developing training materials due to age difference in target groups, coordination with Department of Health to implement training interventions in health cen- ters, participants’ attrition in different steps of the program. Given the findings of the study it is suggested that some more studies should be con- ducted regarding effectiveness of other healthcare models and theories in adopting AIDS protective behaviors. Also theory of planned behavior and other models in adopting AIDS protective behaviors should be compared. Finally, present study is conducted regarding adopting AIDS protective behaviors among health contact persons Table 3. Correlation between constructs of the TPB. Perceived behavioral control Subjective norm Behavioral intention Behavior Attitude toward behavior Knowledge Knowledge Correlation coefficient 1 0/16 0/01 0/30 0/17 0/21 p value 0/20 0/88 0/01 0/18 0/10 Attitude toward behavior Correlation coefficient 0/16 1 0/22 0/11 0/25 0/10 p value 0/20 0/08 0/37 0/04 0/44 Behavior Correlation coefficient 0/01 0/22 1 0/04 0/37 0/07 p value 0/88 0/08 0/71 0/003 0/55 Behavioral intention Correlation coefficient 0/30 0/11 0/04 1 0/38 0/12 p value 0/01 0/37 0/71 0/002 0/33 Subjective norm Correlation coefficient 0/17 0/25 0/37 0/38 1 0/12 p value 0/18 0/04 0/003 0/002 0/33 Perceived behav- ioral control Correlation coefficient 0/21 0/10 0/07 0/12 0/30 1 p value 0/10 0/44 0/55 0/33 0/019 586 H. A. SIUKI ET AL. in Torbat-e Heydarieh city. It is suggested that the same study should be conducted among health contact persons in other cities. The results of the study indicate the impact of training intervention on AIDS protective behaviors based on theoretical structures of theory of planned behavior. Theory of planned behavior is one of the best theories to predict behaviors which were used as main framework in present study. The findings of the study reaf- firmed effectiveness of this health training theory in changing behaviors. Acknowledgements The authors of the study express their sincere gratitude of all authorities of Torbat-e Heydarieh University of Medical Sciences, Deputy of Health and health contact persons who helped us in conducting this research. Disclosure Statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. ORCID Hadi Alizadeh Siuki https://ORCID.org/0000-0003- 2053-3229 Nooshin Peyman http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6218-4787 Mohammad Vahedian-Shahroodi http://orcid.org/0000- 0002-5402-1646 Hadi Tehrani http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8747-8717 References Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal- directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22(5), 453–474. Alizadeh Siuki, H., Zareban, I., Rakhshani, F., SHahraki Poor, M., Hasanzadeh, M., Shamaeian Razavi, N., … Jadgal Kheir, M. (2013). The effect of peer education on preventive behaviours from aids based on theory of planned behavior in high school second grade students in Zahedan-89. Horizon of Medical Sciences, 18(5), 232–241. Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(Pt 4), 471–499. Atwine, B., Cantor-Graae, E., & Bajunirwe, F. (2005). Psychological distress among AIDS orphans in rural Uganda. Social Science & Medicine, 61(3), 555–564. Duflo, E., Dupas, P., & Kremer, M. (2015). Education, HIV, and early fertility: Experimental evidence from Kenya. The American Economic Review, 105(9), 2757–2797. Eggers, S. M., Taylor, M., Sathiparsad, R., Bos, A. E., & de Vries, H. (2015). Predicting safe sex: Assessment of autoregressive and cross-lagged effects within the Theory of Planned Behavior. Journal of Health Psychology, 20(11), 1397–1404. Eslami, H., Marzban, A., & Mazloomy, S. S. (2016). Investigating the Knowledge and Attitude of Health Volunteers about HIV/AIDS in Eghlid, Iran Health Centers in 2015. Journal of Health Research in Community, 2(1), 21–27. Fallahzadeh, H., Morowatisharifabad, M., & Ehrampoosh, M. (2009). HIV/AIDS epidemic features and trends in Iran, 1986–2006. AIDS and Behavior, 13(2), 297–302. Gebreeyesus Hadera, H., Boer, H., & Kuiper, W. A. (2007). Using the theory of planned behaviour to understand the motivation to learn about HIV/AIDS prevention among adolescents in Tigray, Ethiopia. AIDs Care, 19(7), 895–900. Heald, S. (2006). Abstain or die: the development of HIV/ AIDS policy in Botswana. Journal of Biosocial Science, 38(1), 29–41. Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS, & World Health Organization. (2007). AIDS epidemic update, December 2006. Geneva: World Health Organization. Kelly, J. A., & Lawrence, J. S. S. (2013). The AIDS health crisis: Psychological and social interventions. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media. Medley, A., Kennedy, C., O’Reilly, K., & Sweat, M. (2009). Effectiveness of peer education interventions for HIV prevention in developing countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis. AIDS Education and Prevention: Official Publication of the International Society for Aids Education, 21(3), 181–206. Mee, P., Collinson, M. A., Madhavan, S., Kabudula, C., G�omez-Oliv�e, F. X., Kahn, K., … Byass, P. (2014). Determinants of the risk of dying of HIV/AIDS in a rural South African community over the period of the decen- tralised roll-out of antiretroviral therapy: A longitudinal study. Global Health Action, 7(1), 24826. Meister, H., Grugel, L., & Meis, M. (2014). Intention to use hearing aids: a survey based on the theory of planned behavior. Patient Preference and Adherence, 8, 1265. Mohammadi Zeidi, I., Pakpour Hajiagha, A., & Mohammadi Zeidi, B. (2013). Evaluation of educational programs based on the theory of planned behavior on employees’ safety behaviors. Journal of Mazandaran University of Medical Sciences, 22(97), 166–177. Morlat, P., Roussillon, C., Henard, S., Salmon, D., Bonnet, F., Cacoub, P., … Chene, G. (2014). Causes of death among HIV-infected patients in France in 2010 (national survey): trends since 2000. AIDS, 28(8), 1181–1191. Pakpour Hajiagha, A., Mohammadi Zeidi, I., & Mohammadi Zeidi, B. (2012). The impact of health education based on theory of planned behavior on the JOURNAL OF SOCIAL SERVICE RESEARCH 587 prevention of AIDS among adolescents. Iran Journal of Nursing, 25(78), 1–13. Reeves, S., Perrier, L., Goldman, J., Freeth, D., & Zwarenstein, M. (2013). Interprofessional education: Effects on profes- sional practice and healthcare outcomes (update). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 3, CD002213. Sadeghi, R., & Khanjani, N. (2015). Impact of educational intervention based on theory of planned behavior (TPB) on the AIDS-preventive behavior among health volun- teers. Iranian Journal of Health Education and Health Promotion, 3(1), 23–31. Shojaeizadeh, D., Taheri, G. E., & Tehrani, H. (2012). The effect of education on knowledge and attitude of high school students about AIDS in Faruj, Iran. Journal of Health and Development, 1(1), 67–73. Tyson, M., Covey, J., & Rosenthal, H. E. (2014). Theory of planned behavior interventions for reducing heterosexual risk behaviors: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 33(12), 1454. UNICEF, Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS, & World Health Organization. (2002). Young people and HIV/AIDS: Opportunity in crisis. The Stationery Office. https://www.unicef.org/publications/index_4447. html Vafaeenajar, A., Masihabadi, M., Moshki, M., Ebrahimipour, H., Tehrani, H., & Esmaily, H. (2015). Determining the theory of planned behavior’s predictive power on adolescents’ dependence on computer games. Iranian Journal of Health Education and Health Promotion, 2(4), 303–311. 588 H. A. SIUKI ET AL. https://www.unicef.org/publications/index_4447.html https://www.unicef.org/publications/index_4447.html Abstract Introduction Method Study Design and Study Target … NIH Publication No. 05-3896 Printed September 2005 2931-NCI Theory cvr.f 11/17/05 3:18 PM Page 1 Theor y at a Gl ance U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES National Institutes of Health A G u i d e F o r H e a l t h P ro m o t i o n P ra c t i ce Theory at a Glance A Guide For Health Promotion Practice (Second Edition) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES National Institutes of Health Foreword A decade ago, the first edition of Theory at a Glance was published. The guide was a welcome resource for public health practitioners seeking a single, concise summary of health behavior theories that was neither overwhelming nor superficial. As a government publication in the public domain, it also provided cash-strapped health departments with access to a seminal integration of scholarly work that was useful to program staff, interns, and directors alike. Although they were not the primary target audience, members of the public health research community also utilized Theory at a Glance, both as a quick desk reference and as a primer for their students. The National Cancer Institute is pleased to sponsor the publication of this guide, but its relevance is by no means limited to cancer prevention and control. The principles described herein can serve as frameworks for many domains of public health intervention, complementing focused evidence reviews such as Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Guide to Community Preventive Services. This report also complements a number of other efforts by NCI and our federal partners to facilitate more rigorous testing and application of health behavior theories through training workshops and the development of new Web-based resources. One reason theory is so useful is that it helps us articulate assumptions and hypotheses concerning our strategies and targets of intervention. Debates among policymakers concerning public health programs are often complicated by unspoken assumptions or confusion about which data are relevant. Theory can inform these debates by clarifying key constructs and their presumed relationships. Especially when the evidence base is small, advocates of one approach or another can be challenged to address the mechanisms by which a program is expected to have an impact. By specifying these alternative pathways to change, program evaluations can be designed to ensure that regardless of the outcome, improvements in knowledge, program design, and implementation will occur. I am pleased to introduce this second edition of Theory at a Glance. I am especially impressed that the lead authors, Dr. Barbara K. Rimer and Dr. Karen Glanz, have enhanced and updated it throughout without diminishing the clarity and efficiency of the original. We hope that this new edition will empower another generation of public health practitioners to apply the same conceptual rigor to program planning and design that these authors exemplify in their own research and practice. Robert T. Croyle, Ph.D. Director Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences National Cancer Institute Spring 2005 Acknowledgements The National Cancer Institute would like to thank Barbara Rimer Dr.P.H. and Karen Glanz Ph.D., M.P.H., authors of the original monograph, whose knowledge of healthcommunications theory and practice have molded a generation of health promotion practitioners. Both have provided hours of review and consultation, and we are grateful to them for their contributions. Thanks to the staffs of the Office of Communications, particularly Margaret Farrell, and the Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences and Kelly Blake, who guided this monograph to completion. We appreciate in particular the work of Karen Harris, whose attention to detail and commitment to excellence enhanced the monograph’s content and quality. Table of Contents Introduction viii Audience and Purpose 1 Contents 1 Part 1: Foundations of Theory in Health Promotion and Health Behavior 3 Why Is Theory Important to Health Promotion and Health Behavior Practice? 4 What Is Theory? 4 How Can Theory Help Plan Effective Programs? 4 Explanatory Theory and Change Theory 5 Fitting Theory to the Field of Practice 5 Using Theory to Address Health Issues in Diverse Populations 7 Part 2: Theories and Applications 9 The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach 10 Theoretical Explanations of Three Levels of Influence 12 Individual or Intrapersonal Level 12 Health Belief Model 13 Stages of Change Model 15 Theory of Planned Behavior 16 Precaution Adoption Process Model 18 Interpersonal Level 19 Social Cognitive Theory 19 Community Level 22 Community Organization and Other Participatory Models 23 Diffusion of Innovations 27 Communication Theory 29 Media Effects 30 Agenda Setting 30 New Communication Technologies 31 Part 3: Putting Theory and Practice Together 35 Planning Models 36 Social Marketing 36 PRECEDE-PROCEED 39 Where to Begin: Choosing the Right Theories 43 A Few Final Words 44 Sources 48 References 49 Tables and Figures Tables Table 1 An Ecological Perspective: Levels of Influence 11 Table 2 Health Belief Model 14 Table 3 Stages of Change Model 15 Table 4 Theory of Planned Behavior 17 Table 5 Social Cognitive Theory 20 Table 6 Community Organization 24 Table 7 Concepts in Diffusion of Innovations 27 Table 8 Key Attributes Affecting the Speed and Extent of an Innovation’s Diffusion 28 Table 9 Agenda Setting, Concepts, Definitions, and Applications 31 Table 10 Diagnostic Elements of PRECEDE-PROCEED 42 Table 11 Summary of Theories: Focus and Key Concepts 45 Figures Figure 1 Using Explanatory Theory and Change Theory to Plan and Evaluate Programs 6 Figure 2 A Multilevel Approach to Epidemiology 10 Figure 3 Theory of Reasoned Action and Theory of Planned Behavior 18 Figure 4 Stages of the Precaution Adoption Process Model 19 Figure 5 An Integrative Model 21 Figure 6 Sociocultural Environment Logic Framework 26 Figure 7 An Asthma Self-Management Video Game for Children 33 Figure 8 Social Marketing Wheel 38 Figure 9 The PRECEDE-PROCEED Model 40 Figure 10 Using Theory to Plan Multilevel Interventions 46 Introduction viii T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E T his monograph, Theory at a Glance: Application to Health Promotion and Health Behavior (Second Edition), describes influential theories of health-related behaviors, processes of shaping behavior, and the effects of community and environmental factors on behavior. It complements existing resources that offer tools, techniques, and model programs for practice, such as Making Health Communication Programs Work: A Planner’s Guide,i and the Web portal, Cancer Control PLANET (Plan, Link, Act, Network with Evidence-based Tools).ii Theory at a Glance makes health behavior theory accessible and provides tools to solve problems and assess the effectiveness of health promotion programs. (For the purposes of this monograph, health promotion is broadly defined as the process of enabling people to increase control over, and to improve, their health. Thus, the focus goes beyond traditional primary and secondary prevention programs.) For nearly a decade, public health and health care practitioners have consulted the original version of Theory at a Glance for guidance on using theories about human behavior to inform program planning, implementation, and evaluation. We have received many testimonials about the First Edition’s usefulness, and requests for additional copies. This updated edition includes information from recent health behavior research and suggests theoretical approaches to developing programs for diverse populations. Theory at a Glance can be used as a stand-alone handbook, as part of in-house staff development programs, or in conjunction with theory texts and continuing education workshops. For easy reference, the monograph includes only a small number of current and applicable health behavior theories. The theories reviewed here are widely used for the purposes of cancer control, defining risk, and segmenting populations. Much of the content for this publication has been adapted from the third edition of Glanz, Rimer, and Lewis’ Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice,1 published by Jossey-Bass in San Francisco. Readers who want to learn more about useful theories for health behavior change and health education practice can consult this and other sources that are recommended in the References section at the end of the monograph. i Making Health Communication Programs Work (http://www.nci.nih.gov/pinkbook/) describes a practical approach for planning and implementing health communication efforts. ii Cancer Control PLANET (http://cancercontrolplanet.cancer.gov) provides access to data and resources that can help planners, program staff, and researchers to design, implement, and evaluate evidence-based cancer control programs. (http://www.nci.nih.gov/pinkbook/) (http://cancercontrolplanet.cancer.gov) Audience and Purpose This monograph is written primarily for public health workers in state and local health agencies; it is also valuable for health promotion practitioners and volunteers who work in voluntary health agencies, community organizations, health care settings, schools, and the private sector. Interventions based on health behavior theory are not guaranteed to succeed, but they are much more likely to produce desired outcomes. Theory at a Glance is designed to help users understand how individuals, groups, and organizations behave and change—knowledge they can use to design effective programs. For information about specific, evidence-based interventions to promote health and prevent disease, readers may also wish to consult the Guide to Community Preventive Services, published by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) at www.thecommunityguide.org. Contents This monograph consists of three parts. For each theory, the text highlights key concepts and their applications. These summaries may be used as “checklists” of important issues to consider when planning or evaluating programs or to prompt project teams to think about the range of factors that influence health behavior. Part 1. Foundations of Theory in Health Promotion and Health Behavior describes ways that theories and models can be useful in health behavior/health promotion practice and provides basic definitions. Part 2. Theories and Applications presents an ecological perspective on health behavior/health promotion programs. It describes eight theories and models that explain individual, interpersonal, and community behavior and offers approaches to solving problems. A brief description of each theory is followed by definitions of key concepts and examples or case studies. The section also explores the use of new communication technologies. Part 3. Putting Theory and Practice Together explains how theory can be used in health behavior/health promotion program planning, implementation, and evaluation. Two comprehensive planning models, PRECEDE-PROCEED and social marketing, are reviewed. 1 IN TR O T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E http:www.thecommunityguide.org Part 1 Foundations of Theory in Health Promotion and Health Behavior 3 PA R T 1 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E 4 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E Why Is Theory Important to Health Promotion and Health Behavior Practice? Effective public health, health promotion, and chronic disease management programs help people maintain and improve health, reduce disease risks, and manage chronic illness. They can improve the well-being and self-sufficiency of individuals, families, organizations, and communities. Usually, such successes require behavior change at many levels, (e.g., individual, organizational, and community). Not all health programs and initiatives are equally successful, however. Those most likely to achieve desired outcomes are based on a clear understanding of targeted health behaviors, and the environmental context in which they occur. Practitioners use strategic planning models to develop and manage these programs, and continually improve them through meaningful evaluation. Health behavior theory can play a critical role throughout the program planning process. What Is Theory? A theory presents a systematic way of understanding events or situations. It is a set of concepts, definitions, and propositions that explain or predict these events or situations by illustrating the relationships between variables. Theories must be applicable to a broad variety of situations. They are, by nature, abstract, and don’t have a specified content or topic area. Like empty coffee cups, theories have shapes and boundaries, but nothing inside. They become useful when filled with practical topics, goals, and problems. • Concepts are the building blocks—the primary elements—of a theory. • Constructs are concepts developed or adopted for use in a particular theory. The key concepts of a given theory are its constructs. • Variables are the operational forms of constructs. They define the way a construct is to be measured in a specific situation. Match variables to constructs when identifying what needs to be assessed during evaluation of a theory- driven program. • Models may draw on a number of theories to help understand a particular problem in a certain setting or context. They are not always as specified as theory. Most health behavior and health promotion theories were adapted from the social and behavioral sciences, but applying them to health issues often requires that one be familiar with epidemiology and the biological sciences. Health behavior and health promotion theories draw upon various disciplines, such as psychology, sociology, anthropology, consumer behavior, and marketing. Many are not highly developed or have not been rigorously tested. Because of this, they often are called conceptual frameworks or theoretical frameworks; here the terms are used interchangeably. How Can Theory Help Plan Effective Programs? Theory gives planners tools for moving beyond intuition to design and evaluate health behavior and health promotion interventions based on understanding of behavior. It helps them to step back and consider the larger picture. Like an artist, a program planner who grounds health interventions in theory creates innovative ways to address specific circumstances. He or she does not depend on a “paint-by­ numbers” approach, re-hashing stale ideas, but uses a palette of behavior theories, skillfully applying them to develop unique, tailored solutions to problems. Using theory as a foundation for program planning and development is consistent with the current emphasis on using evidence- based interventions in public health, behavioral medicine, and medicine. Theory provides a road map for studying problems, developing appropriate interventions, and evaluating their successes. It can inform the planner’s thinking during all of these stages, offering insights that translate into stronger programs. Theory can also help to explain the dynamics of health behaviors, including processes for changing them, and the influences of the many forces that affect health behaviors, including social and physical environments. Theory can also help planners identify the most suitable target audiences, methods for fostering change, and outcomes for evaluation. Researchers and practitioners use theory to investigate answers to the questions of “why,” “what,” and “how” health problems should be addressed. By seeking answers to these questions, they clarify the nature of targeted health behaviors. That is, theory guides the search for reasons why people do or do not engage in certain health behaviors; it helps pinpoint what planners need to know before they develop public health programs; and it suggests how to devise program strategies that reach target audiences and have an impact. Theory also helps to identify which indicators should be monitored and measured during program evaluation. For these reasons, program planning, implementation, and monitoring processes based in theory are more likely to succeed than those developed without the benefit of a theoretical perspective. Explanatory Theory and Change Theory Explanatory theory describes the reasons why a problem exists. It guides the search for factors that contribute to a problem (e.g., a lack of knowledge, self-efficacy, social support, or resources), and can be changed. Examples of explanatory theories include the Health Belief Model, the Theory of Planned Behavior, and the Precaution Adoption Process Model. Change theory guides the development of health interventions. It spells out concepts that can be translated into program messages and strategies, and offers a basis for program evaluation. Change theory helps program planners to be explicit about their assumptions for why a program will work. Examples of change theories include Community Organization and Diffusion of Innovations. Figure 1. illustrates how explanatory theory and change theory can be used to plan and evaluate programs. Fitting Theory to the Field of Practice This monograph includes descriptions and applications of some theories that are central to health behavior and health promotion practice today. No single theory dominates health education and promotion, nor should it; the problems, behaviors, populations, cultures, and contexts of public health practice are broad and varied. Some theories focus on individuals as the unit of change. Others examine change within families, institutions, communities, or cultures. Adequately addressing an issue may require more than one theory, and no one theory is suitable for all cases. 5 PA R T 1 FO U N D A T IO N S O F A P P LY IN G T H E O R Y IN H E A LT H P R O M O T IO N P R A C T IC E Figure 1. Using Explanatory Theory and Change Theory to Plan and Evaluate Programs Problem Behavior or Situation ChangeTheory Which strategies? Which messages? Assumptions about how a program should work Evaluation Planning Explanatory Theory Why? What can be changed? 6 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E Because the social context in which behavior occurs is always evolving, theories that were important in public health education a generation ago may be of limited use today. At the same time, new social science research allows theorists to refine and adapt existing theories. A recent Institute of Medicine report2 observed that several theorists have converged in their views, identifying several variables as central to behavior change. As a result, some constructs, such as self-efficacy, are central to multiple theories. Effective practice depends on using theories and strategies that are appropriate to a situation. One of the greatest challenges for those concerned with behavior change is learning to analyze how well a theory or model “fits” a particular issue. A working knowledge of specific theories, and familiarity with how they have been applied in the past, improves skills in this area. Selecting an appropriate theory or combination of theories helps take into account the multiple factors that influence health behaviors. The practitioner who uses theory develops a nuanced understanding of realistic program outcomes that drives the planning process. Choosing a theory that will bring a useful perspective to the problem at hand does not begin with a theory (e.g., the most familiar theory, the theory mentioned in a recent journal article, etc.). Instead, this process starts with a thorough assessment of the situation: the units of analysis or change, the topic, and the type of behavior to be addressed. Because different theoretical frameworks are appropriate and practical for different situations, selecting a theory that “fits” should be a careful, deliberate process. Start with the steps in the box at the top of the next page. A Good Fit: Characteristics of a Useful Theory A useful theory makes assumptions about a behavior, health problem, target population, or environment that are: • Logical; • Consistent with everyday observations; • Similar to those used in previous successful programs; and • Supported by past research in the same area or related ideas. Using Theory to Address Health Issues in Diverse Populations The U.S. population is growing more culturally and ethnically diverse. An increasing body of research shows health disparities exist among various ethnic and socio-economic groups. These findings highlight the importance of understanding the cultural backgrounds and life experiences of community members, though research has not yet established when and under what circumstances targeted or tailored health communications are more effective than generic ones. (Targeting involves using information about shared characteristics of a population subgroup to create a single intervention approach for that group. In contrast, tailoring is a process that uses an assessment to derive information about one specific person, and then offers change or information strategies for an outcome of interest based on that person’s unique characteristics.)3 Most health behavior theories can be applied to diverse cultural and ethnic groups, but health practitioners must understand the characteristics of target populations (e.g., ethnicity, socioeconomic status, gender, age, and geographical location) to use these theories correctly. There are several reasons why culture and ethnicity are critical to consider when applying theory to a health problem. First, morbidity and mortality rates for different diseases vary by race and ethnicity; second, there are differences in the prevalence of risk behaviors among these groups; and third, the determinants of health behaviors vary across racial and ethnic groups. What People in the Field Say About Theory “Theory is different from most of the tools I use in my work. It’s more abstract, but that can be a plus too. A solid grounding in a handful of theories goes a long way toward helping me think through why I approach a health problem the way I do.” — County Health Educator “I used to think theory was just for students and researchers. But now I have a better grasp of it; I appreciate how practical it can be.” — State Chronic Disease Administrator “By translating concepts from theory into real-world terms, I can get my staff and community volunteers to take a closer look at why we’re conducting programs the way we do, and how they can succeed or fail.” — City Tobacco Control Coordinator “A good grasp of theory is essential for leadership. It gives you a broader way of viewing your work. And it helps create a vision for the future. But, of course, it’s only worthwhile if I can translate it clearly and simply to my co-workers.” — Regional Health Promotion Chief “It’s not as hard as I thought it would be to keep up with current theories. More than ever these days, there are tools and workshops to update us often.” — Patient Education Coordinator 7 PA R T 1 FO U N D A T IO N S O F A P P LY IN G T H E O R Y IN H E A LT H P R O M O T IO N P R A C T IC E Part 2 Theories and Applications 9 PA R T 2 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach Contemporary health promotion involves more than simply educating individuals about healthy practices. It includes efforts to change organizational behavior, as well as the physical and social environment of communities. It is also about developing and advocating for policies that support health, such as economic incentives. Health promotion programs that seek to address health problems across this spectrum employ a range of strategies, and operate on multiple levels. The ecological perspective emphasizes the interaction between, and interdependence of, factors within and across all levels of a health problem. It highlights people’s interactions with their physical and socio­ cultural environments. Two key concepts of the ecological perspective help to identify intervention points for promoting health: first, behavior both affects, and is affected by, multiple levels of influence; second, individual behavior both shapes, and is shaped by, the social environment (reciprocal causation). To explain the first key concept of the ecological perspective, multiple levels of influence, McLeroy and colleagues (1988)4 identified five levels of influence for health- related behaviors and conditions. Defined in Table 1., these levels include: (1) intrapersonal or individual factors; (2) interpersonal factors; (3) institutional or organizational factors; (4) community factors; and (5) public policy factors. 10 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E Figure 2. A Multilevel Approach to Epidemiology Social and Economic Policies Institutions Neighborhoods and Communities Living Conditions Social Relationships Individual Risk Factors Pathophysiological Pathways Individual/Population Health Genetic/Constitutional Factors Envir onm ent Li fe co ur se Source: Smedley BD, Syme SL (eds.), Institute of Medicine. Promoting Health: Strategies from Social and Behavioral Research. Washington, D.C.:, National Academies Press, 2000. Table 1. An Ecological Perspective: Levels of Influence Concept Intrapersonal Level Interpersonal Level Community Level Institutional Factors Community Factors Public Policy Definition Individual characteristics that influence behavior, such as knowledge, attitudes, beliefs, and personality traits Interpersonal processes and primary groups, including family, friends, and peers that provide social identity, support, and role definition Rules, regulations, policies, and informal structures, which may constrain or promote recommended behaviors Social networks and norms, or standards, which exist as formal or informal among individuals, groups, and organizations Local, state, and federal policies and laws that regulate or support healthy actions and practices for disease prevention, early detection, control, and management In practice, addressing the community level requires taking into consideration institutional and public policy factors, as well as social networks and norms. Figure 2. illustrates how different levels of influence combine to affect population health. Each level of influence can affect health behavior. For example, suppose a woman delays getting a recommended mammogram (screening for breast cancer). At the individual level, her inaction may be due to fears of finding out she has cancer. At the interpersonal level, her doctor may neglect to tell her that she should get the test, or she may have friends who say they do not believe it is important to get a mammogram. At the organizational level, it may be hard to schedule an appointment, because there is only a part-time radiologist at the clinic. At the policy level, she may lack insurance coverage, and thus be unable to afford the fee. Thus, the outcome, the woman’s failure to get a mammogram, may result from multiple factors. The second key concept of an ecological perspective, reciprocal causation, suggests that people both influence, and are influenced by, those around them. For example, a man with high cholesterol may find it hard to follow the diet his doctor has prescribed because his company cafeteria doesn’t offer healthy food choices. To comply with his doctor’s instructions, he can try to change the environment by asking the cafeteria manager to add healthy items to the menu, or he can dine elsewhere. If he and enough of his fellow employees decide to find someplace else to eat, the cafeteria may change its menu to maintain lunch business. Thus, the cafeteria environment may compel this man to change his dining habits, but his new habits may ultimately bring about change in the cafeteria as well. 11 PA R T 2 T H E O R IE S A N D A P P LIC A T IO N S 12 T H E O R Y A T A G LA N C E An ecological perspective shows the advantages of multilevel interventions that combine behavioral and environmental components. For instance, effective tobacco control programs often use multiple strategies to discourage smoking.5 Employee smoking cessation clinics have a stronger impact if the workplace has a no-smoking policy and the city has a clean indoor air ordinance. Adolescents are less likely to begin smoking if their peers disapprove of the habit and laws prohibiting tobacco sales to minors are strictly enforced. Health promotion programs are more effective when planners consider multiple levels of influence on health problems. Theoretical Explanations of Three Levels of …
CATEGORIES
Economics Nursing Applied Sciences Psychology Science Management Computer Science Human Resource Management Accounting Information Systems English Anatomy Operations Management Sociology Literature Education Business & Finance Marketing Engineering Statistics Biology Political Science Reading History Financial markets Philosophy Mathematics Law Criminal Architecture and Design Government Social Science World history Chemistry Humanities Business Finance Writing Programming Telecommunications Engineering Geography Physics Spanish ach e. Embedded Entrepreneurship f. Three Social Entrepreneurship Models g. Social-Founder Identity h. Micros-enterprise Development Outcomes Subset 2. Indigenous Entrepreneurship Approaches (Outside of Canada) a. Indigenous Australian Entrepreneurs Exami Calculus (people influence of  others) processes that you perceived occurs in this specific Institution Select one of the forms of stratification highlighted (focus on inter the intersectionalities  of these three) to reflect and analyze the potential ways these ( American history Pharmacology Ancient history . Also Numerical analysis Environmental science Electrical Engineering Precalculus Physiology Civil Engineering Electronic Engineering ness Horizons Algebra Geology Physical chemistry nt When considering both O lassrooms Civil Probability ions Identify a specific consumer product that you or your family have used for quite some time. This might be a branded smartphone (if you have used several versions over the years) or the court to consider in its deliberations. Locard’s exchange principle argues that during the commission of a crime Chemical Engineering Ecology aragraphs (meaning 25 sentences or more). Your assignment may be more than 5 paragraphs but not less. INSTRUCTIONS:  To access the FNU Online Library for journals and articles you can go the FNU library link here:  https://www.fnu.edu/library/ In order to n that draws upon the theoretical reading to explain and contextualize the design choices. Be sure to directly quote or paraphrase the reading ce to the vaccine. Your campaign must educate and inform the audience on the benefits but also create for safe and open dialogue. A key metric of your campaign will be the direct increase in numbers.  Key outcomes: The approach that you take must be clear Mechanical Engineering Organic chemistry Geometry nment Topic You will need to pick one topic for your project (5 pts) Literature search You will need to perform a literature search for your topic Geophysics you been involved with a company doing a redesign of business processes Communication on Customer Relations. Discuss how two-way communication on social media channels impacts businesses both positively and negatively. Provide any personal examples from your experience od pressure and hypertension via a community-wide intervention that targets the problem across the lifespan (i.e. includes all ages). Develop a community-wide intervention to reduce elevated blood pressure and hypertension in the State of Alabama that in in body of the report Conclusions References (8 References Minimum) *** Words count = 2000 words. *** In-Text Citations and References using Harvard style. *** In Task section I’ve chose (Economic issues in overseas contracting)" Electromagnetism w or quality improvement; it was just all part of good nursing care.  The goal for quality improvement is to monitor patient outcomes using statistics for comparison to standards of care for different diseases e a 1 to 2 slide Microsoft PowerPoint presentation on the different models of case management.  Include speaker notes... .....Describe three different models of case management. visual representations of information. They can include numbers SSAY ame workbook for all 3 milestones. You do not need to download a new copy for Milestones 2 or 3. When you submit Milestone 3 pages): Provide a description of an existing intervention in Canada making the appropriate buying decisions in an ethical and professional manner. Topic: Purchasing and Technology You read about blockchain ledger technology. Now do some additional research out on the Internet and share your URL with the rest of the class be aware of which features their competitors are opting to include so the product development teams can design similar or enhanced features to attract more of the market. The more unique low (The Top Health Industry Trends to Watch in 2015) to assist you with this discussion.         https://youtu.be/fRym_jyuBc0 Next year the $2.8 trillion U.S. healthcare industry will   finally begin to look and feel more like the rest of the business wo evidence-based primary care curriculum. Throughout your nurse practitioner program Vignette Understanding Gender Fluidity Providing Inclusive Quality Care Affirming Clinical Encounters Conclusion References Nurse Practitioner Knowledge Mechanics and word limit is unit as a guide only. The assessment may be re-attempted on two further occasions (maximum three attempts in total). All assessments must be resubmitted 3 days within receiving your unsatisfactory grade. You must clearly indicate “Re-su Trigonometry Article writing Other 5. June 29 After the components sending to the manufacturing house 1. In 1972 the Furman v. Georgia case resulted in a decision that would put action into motion. Furman was originally sentenced to death because of a murder he committed in Georgia but the court debated whether or not this was a violation of his 8th amend One of the first conflicts that would need to be investigated would be whether the human service professional followed the responsibility to client ethical standard.  While developing a relationship with client it is important to clarify that if danger or Ethical behavior is a critical topic in the workplace because the impact of it can make or break a business No matter which type of health care organization With a direct sale During the pandemic Computers are being used to monitor the spread of outbreaks in different areas of the world and with this record 3. Furman v. Georgia is a U.S Supreme Court case that resolves around the Eighth Amendments ban on cruel and unsual punishment in death penalty cases. The Furman v. Georgia case was based on Furman being convicted of murder in Georgia. Furman was caught i One major ethical conflict that may arise in my investigation is the Responsibility to Client in both Standard 3 and Standard 4 of the Ethical Standards for Human Service Professionals (2015).  Making sure we do not disclose information without consent ev 4. Identify two examples of real world problems that you have observed in your personal Summary & Evaluation: Reference & 188. Academic Search Ultimate Ethics We can mention at least one example of how the violation of ethical standards can be prevented. Many organizations promote ethical self-regulation by creating moral codes to help direct their business activities *DDB is used for the first three years For example The inbound logistics for William Instrument refer to purchase components from various electronic firms. During the purchase process William need to consider the quality and price of the components. In this case 4. A U.S. Supreme Court case known as Furman v. Georgia (1972) is a landmark case that involved Eighth Amendment’s ban of unusual and cruel punishment in death penalty cases (Furman v. Georgia (1972) With covid coming into place In my opinion with Not necessarily all home buyers are the same! When you choose to work with we buy ugly houses Baltimore & nationwide USA The ability to view ourselves from an unbiased perspective allows us to critically assess our personal strengths and weaknesses. This is an important step in the process of finding the right resources for our personal learning style. Ego and pride can be · By Day 1 of this week While you must form your answers to the questions below from our assigned reading material CliftonLarsonAllen LLP (2013) 5 The family dynamic is awkward at first since the most outgoing and straight forward person in the family in Linda Urien The most important benefit of my statistical analysis would be the accuracy with which I interpret the data. The greatest obstacle From a similar but larger point of view 4 In order to get the entire family to come back for another session I would suggest coming in on a day the restaurant is not open When seeking to identify a patient’s health condition After viewing the you tube videos on prayer Your paper must be at least two pages in length (not counting the title and reference pages) The word assimilate is negative to me. I believe everyone should learn about a country that they are going to live in. It doesnt mean that they have to believe that everything in America is better than where they came from. It means that they care enough Data collection Single Subject Chris is a social worker in a geriatric case management program located in a midsize Northeastern town. She has an MSW and is part of a team of case managers that likes to continuously improve on its practice. The team is currently using an I would start off with Linda on repeating her options for the child and going over what she is feeling with each option.  I would want to find out what she is afraid of.  I would avoid asking her any “why” questions because I want her to be in the here an Summarize the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psychological research (Comp 2.1) 25.0\% Summarization of the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psych Identify the type of research used in a chosen study Compose a 1 Optics effect relationship becomes more difficult—as the researcher cannot enact total control of another person even in an experimental environment. Social workers serve clients in highly complex real-world environments. Clients often implement recommended inte I think knowing more about you will allow you to be able to choose the right resources Be 4 pages in length soft MB-920 dumps review and documentation and high-quality listing pdf MB-920 braindumps also recommended and approved by Microsoft experts. The practical test g One thing you will need to do in college is learn how to find and use references. References support your ideas. College-level work must be supported by research. You are expected to do that for this paper. You will research Elaborate on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study 20.0\% Elaboration on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study is missing. Elaboration on any potenti 3 The first thing I would do in the family’s first session is develop a genogram of the family to get an idea of all the individuals who play a major role in Linda’s life. After establishing where each member is in relation to the family A Health in All Policies approach Note: The requirements outlined below correspond to the grading criteria in the scoring guide. At a minimum Chen Read Connecting Communities and Complexity: A Case Study in Creating the Conditions for Transformational Change Read Reflections on Cultural Humility Read A Basic Guide to ABCD Community Organizing Use the bolded black section and sub-section titles below to organize your paper. For each section Losinski forwarded the article on a priority basis to Mary Scott Losinksi wanted details on use of the ED at CGH. He asked the administrative resident