REF 7 - Psychology
Write a reflection paper. add your own thoughts. and stories or experiences 2 page. Infant Behavior and Development 64 (2021) 101617 Available online 30 July 2021 0163-6383/© 2021 Published by Elsevier Inc. Review The role of redundant verbal labels in 8- and 10-month-olds’ working memory Allison Fitch a,*, Nilam Thaker b, Zsuzsa Kaldy b a Rochester Institute of Technology, United States b University of Massachusetts Boston, United States A R T I C L E I N F O Keywords: Working memory Label Object Multimodal A B S T R A C T Verbal labels have been shown to help preverbal infants’ performance on various cognitive tasks, such as categorization. Redundant labels also aid adults’ visual working memory (WM), but it is not known if this linguistic benefit extends to preverbal infants’ WM. In two eye-tracking studies, we tested whether 8- and 10-month-old infants’ WM performance would improve with the presence of redundant labels in a Delayed Match Retrieval (DMR) paradigm that tested infants’ WM for object-location bindings. Findings demonstrated that infants at both ages were unable to remember two object-location bindings when co-presented with labels at encoding. Moreover, infants who encoded the object-location bindings with labels were not significantly better than those who did so in silence. These findings are discussed in the context of label advantages in cognition and auditory dominance. 1. Introduction In the domain of cognition, language holds a privileged status among other auditory and communicative signals. This is evident early in life, before children learn to produce language. Infants as young as three months are able to form object categories from labeling phrases but not from tones (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Ferry et al., 2010). At seven months, infants are able to abstract rules from speech sounds but not from tones (Marcus et al., 1999). At one year, infants are able to use labels but not emotional expression to individuate objects (Xu et al., 2005), and show enhanced activation in visual cortices to objects with a label they know relative to objects with a label they have yet to acquire (Gliga et al., 2010). Infants go on to demonstrate the ability to extend a category from a single labeled exemplar (Pomiechowska & Gliga, 2019), and by age 2–3 years, labels facilitate analogical reasoning (Christie & Gentner, 2014) and visual search (Vales & Smith, 2015). Despite this breadth of research, fewer studies have examined the role of labels in infants’ working memory (WM), particularly when these labels are redundant. This paucity is surprising, because adults use redundant labels when memorizing an array of visual items, and do so virtually automatically (e.g. Baddeley et al., 1984; Stefurak & Boynton, 1986; Donkin et al., 2015). Consider the game Simon (Hasbro), in which four colored panels illuminate in increasingly longer sequences for a player to repeat by tapping. The player might label the light color “blue…green…red…yellow…” in their head or aloud as they appear in efforts to better encode the order of the colored lights. While it is clear that infants do not engage in this kind of verbal rehearsal, it is plausible that labels might confer memory benefits in the same manner that they confer benefits in other cognitive tasks. In the current study we investigated the role of labeling in WM in very early language learners: 8- and 10-month-old infants, who have * Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Fitch). Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/inbede https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101617 Received 1 June 2020; Received in revised form 19 July 2021; Accepted 25 July 2021 mailto:[email protected] www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/01636383 https://www.elsevier.com/locate/inbede https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101617 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101617 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101617&domain=pdf https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101617 Infant Behavior and Development 64 (2021) 101617 2 only just acquired the first words in their receptive vocabulary (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012). Without labels, infants’ WM is both fragile and limited. At 6 months, infants can only remember the identity of a single object (Kaldy & Leslie, 2005; Kwon et al., 2014). Between 8 and 10 months, they are able to remember both the identity and location of two objects (depending on the maintenance interval; Kaldy & Leslie, 2005; Kaldy, Guillory & Blaser (2016); Kwon et al., 2014; Oakes et al., 2006), and by 12 months can remember the identity of three distinct objects (Feigenson & Carey, 2003; Feigenson & Halberda, 2008). These memory representations are fragile—when WM capacity is overloaded, the representations degrade rapidly in a process called catastrophic forgetting. That is, a 13-month-old may remember three items, but if presented with a fourth, will lose all retention of the first three items (Barner et al., 2007; Feigenson & Carey, 2003; 2005). Given these limitations, infant WM stands to benefit from additional cues like object labels, which may strengthen an individual representation and/or allow them to overcome a capacity limit. A small number of studies have addressed role of labels on infants’ memory, and they have demonstrated success from a young age. Two such studies have shown that young children can use labels to reduce memory demands for object identities via chunking. At 14 months, infants were able to use labels to chunk an array of four visually identical items into two pairs, leading to successful memory for four items despite their putative three-item capacity (Feigenson & Halberda, 2008). Moreover, Kibbe & Feigenson (2014) showed that 2–3-year-old toddlers are able to use labels to recode object arrays into a single chunk, thereby increasing their WM capacity to as many as five items. However, it is currently unknown if labeling benefits extend beyond chunking, which is not always a viable memory strategy. In addition to object identity, language has been shown to aid memory for location in young children. By 3–4 years, use of spatial terms led to improved performance on memory for an object’s location—this was true even if the children did not know the meaning of the label (Dessalegn & Landau, 2008, 2013; Loewenstein & Gentner, 2005). The current study differs from past work on labels in infants’ memory in a few key ways. The youngest children tested in prior work were 14 months, and presumably speaking at least one word. The focus of the current study is on even younger infants, 8- and 10- month-olds, who have only a small receptive vocabulary (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012). Prior work on object identity has also been limited to the use of labels for chunking (Feigenson & Halberda, 2008; Kibbe & Feigenson, 2014), whereas we focus here on redundant labels that add no additional information. Finally, prior work has assessed memory for an object’s identity (Feigenson & Halberda, 2008; Kibbe & Feigenson, 2014) or location (Dessalegn & Landau, 2008, 2013; Loewenstein & Gentner, 2005). In the current para- digm, we assess the successful binding of the object identity and location (“what was where?”). In the context of the literature discussed thus far, we anticipated that labels would improve infants’ WM. However, it was also possible that redundant labels could have negative effects on memory encoding. Work by Robinson, Sloutsky, and their colleagues have argued that labeling is only beneficial to infants under 16 months when held in contrast to nonlinguistic auditory stimuli, and that performance on these cognitive tasks is even better when no auditory stimuli are presented at all (Robinson et al., 2012; Sloutsky & Robinson, 2008). For example, although infants categorize objects better when presented with labels than tones (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Ferry et al., 2010), they also categorize better in a silent condition than a labeled one (Robinson & Sloutsky, 2007). Similar results have been found for object individuation (Robinson & Sloutsky, 2008). This phenomenon, termed auditory dominance, sug- gests that when encoding multimodal stimuli, the visual and auditory systems compete for attentional resources and the auditory system wins (Robinson & Sloutsky, 2019). Infants are unable to direct sufficient resources to processing the visual component of the stimuli, and thus perform more poorly in visual tasks than they would in the absence of labels. The current study examined whether or not redundant labels lead to better performance on a WM task in two groups of infants: 8- and 10-month-olds. Infants in this age range have demonstrated knowledge of a small group of object labels (notably, food and body parts; Bergelson & Swingley, 2012). Additionally, gains in WM capacity occur in this age range, with 10-, but not 8-month-olds, able to hold two object-location bindings in WM on a Delayed Match Retrieval task (DMR; Kaldy et al., 2016; see also Cheng et al., 2019). We conducted two studies to test two predictions on the role of labels in infants’ WM. In Study 1, we tested the effect of redundant labels using labels that infants know (drawn from Bergelson & Swingley, 2012) with 8-month-old infants who were previously shown not to be able to succeed in an object-location WM task without labels. In Study 2, we hypothesized that 10-month-olds, who have shown to be successful without labels in this task, would demonstrate better performance on the DMR with labels. 2. Study 1 Previous research suggests that in typical visual WM tasks, 8-month-olds have a capacity of one object-location binding (Kaldy et al., 2016; Kaldy & Leslie, 2005). A recent eye-tracking study by Kaldy et al. (2016) used the Delayed Match Retrieval (DMR) paradigm to test 8- and 10-month-old infants with two, non-matching, virtual playing cards. Those cards were flipped over, and a third card was presented that matched one of the two from the previous pair. Findings demonstrated that 10-month-old infants made anticipatory looks to the matching card during the delay period, suggesting that they remembered the object-location bindings of each card. Eight-month-old infants did now show a preference to either of the two cards. The goal of Study 1 was to determine if the addition of redundant labels would increase 8-month-olds’ performance on the DMR above chance levels. We adapted the paradigm to include auditory labels that accompanied the presentation of the objects on the cards (and thus, would improve encoding). We hypothesized that unlike in Kaldy et al. (2016), infants in this task would make anticipatory looks to the match card during the delay period, significantly preferring it above the non-match card. 2.1. Method 2.1.1. Participants Participants were 19 healthy, full-term infants (8 females) ranging in age from 7;00–8;24 (M = 7;19; SD = 15.8 days). Two A. Fitch et al. Infant Behavior and Development 64 (2021) 101617 3 additional infants participated, but were excluded for fussiness. For comparison purposes, we also retrieved the data from Kaldy et al. (2016)1 . Their sample included 14 8-months olds (6 females) ranging in age from 7;11− 8;18 (M = 7;30; SD = 13.2 days). The infants in the Kaldy et al. (2016) study comprise the No Labels group, whereas the infants in the current study comprise the Labels group. The sample size was selected based on a power analysis using effect sizes from Kaldy et al. (2016). The reported effect size for the group that performed significantly above chance in the Kaldy et al. (2016) study (10-month-olds) was Cohen’s d = 0.787. With an alpha = .05 and power = .80, the projected sample size required is approximately N = 12 (using G*Power 3.1.9.2; Faul et al., 2007). Consequently, our sample size is adequate for testing whether our sample performed above chance. As for the comparison of two groups (Label/NoLabel), no prior studies can give us benchmark effect sizes, but based on the comparison between 8- and 10-month-olds (d = 1.207) in Kaldy et al. (2016), our current sample sizes would provide above 90 \% power. Families of participants were recruited for the current study from the [redacted] area based on data from state birth records. Families received $20 and a small gift for participation. All of the participants recruited for the current study were from households in which English was spoken >70 \% of the time. Language exposure data were not reported in Kaldy et al. (2016). In compliance with the [redacted] Institutional Review Board, informed consent was obtained from each participant’s legal guardian. 2.1.2. Stimuli Stimuli included virtual depictions of playing cards that contained pictures of familiar objects (an apple, a baby spoon, a baby bottle, and a human hand) on the face side (see Fig. 1a). On any given trial, the apple and spoon were always paired together, and the bottle and hand were always paired together. In addition to being familiar, infants in the 6–9 months age range have been demon- strated to know the labels for these objects (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012). On the back side, the card had a blue texture. Cards were 146 × 146 pixels and subtended a 3.65 degree visual angle. In addition to the visual stimuli, each card was accompanied by a recording of a female voice (recorded by a native English speaker, using infant-directed speech) that labeled the objects on the cards during the presentation of the memory set (e.g. Look at the apple! Look at the spoon!). All speech recordings were controlled for duration. 2.1.3. Apparatus & procedure Eye movements were recorded using a Tobii T120 eye tracker sampling at 60 Hz. Stimuli were displayed on a 17-inch (43.1 cm) screen with a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels. Participants sat on a caregivers’ lap ~60 cm from the eye tracker in a dimly lit testing room. Caregivers were asked to wear a visor covering their eyes to prevent them from viewing the screen. Infants were calibrated to the eye tracker using a standard 5-point-calibration procedure before each block. Infants were presented with a Delayed Match Retrieval paradigm, an anticipatory looking paradigm that measures WM for two object-location bindings (Kaldy et al., 2016). In this task, participants were presented with three virtual playing cards with identical backs but different faces. First, they saw the memory set: two cards that have two different objects on the face. These cards then flipped over and the participants were shown a third, sample card, which was an identical match to one of the two cards in the memory set. After a brief response period, the matching card in the memory set briefly animated (visual reward) and revealed its matching face. Anticipatory looks to the match card during the search period indicate a successful WM representation of the object/location binding. Similarly to the procedure used in Kaldy et al. (2016), infants were first presented with four familiarization trials (Fig. 1b). In these trials, all three cards (the memory set and the sample) entered the screen, but only one card in the memory set (the match) was shown to the infants. Infants were given 2.5 s to encode this object, and it remained face up. During this encoding period, the object on the card’s face was labeled once by a pre-recorded female voice, playing through a centrally located speaker (e.g. Look at the bottle!). Then, the sample card flipped over, revealing its matching face (which was not labeled). After 200 ms, the match card was revealed and was followed by a brief reward animation (for 200 ms). This reward was so brief that infants needed to have made an anticipatory eye movement to the (face-down) match card’s location in order to fixate it; an implicit instruction (‘look for the match to catch the reward animation!’). Then, the matching cards remained face-up and left the screen in an exit sequence. In the exit sequence, the match card moved next to the sample, “kissed” it, and all three cards exited the screen. Each of the four stimuli served as the sample/match card on one of the training trials; order was counterbalanced. These familiarization trials were followed by 10 test trials (see Fig. 2), which began in a similar manner with all three cards entering the screen. This time, both cards in the memory set were flipped sequentially, such that the first card was presented for 2.5 s, then the second card was presented for 2.5 s, then both cards were flipped back over. We used this sequential presentation method to make sure that infants fixated both of the cards during this phase. As with training trials, the presentation of each card in the memory set coincided with an auditory labeling sentence (e.g. Look at the apple!). All three cards remained face-down for 1 s before the sample card was revealed (similarly to the familiarization trials, unlabeled). The sample presentation was followed by a 4 s response period in which participants’ anticipatory looks to both (face-down) cards in the memory set were measured. At the end of the response period, the match card was briefly exposed, followed by the brief visual reward sequence, and the same exit sequence as in the familiarization trials. All trials (both familiarization and test) had a gaze-contingent onset. Trials were preceded by an attention-getter (a spinning fixation cross at the center of the screen), which infants looked at in order to start each trial. The stimulus pairs (apple-spoon, bottle- hand), which card served as the sample/match, and the location of the match (left or right side) were all counterbalanced. Trial order was randomized through Tobii Studio, which uses a Latin square procedure. 1 There were a few minor differences in the methods of these two studies (most notably, in the stimulus set), which we will return to in our Discussion. A. Fitch et al. Infant Behavior and Development 64 (2021) 101617 4 2.1.4. Data analysis We defined three same-size square AOIs (292 × 292 pixels) centered around the two cards in the memory set as well as the sample card. Time to first fixation and total look duration to each AOI were measured during the 4 s response period in each test trial. From these measures we calculated two main variables of interest: First Look and Proportion of Looking Time to the Match. First Look was calculated by comparing the time to first fixation on the match and non-match card, and determining which occurred first. The number of trials in which a participant made their first look to the match was then summed and divided by the number of trials to get a proportion (values range from 0 to 1, with 1 meaning all first looks were to the match card). Proportion Looking Time was calculated trial-by-trial by dividing the total look duration to the match card by the sum total look duration to either the match or nonmatch card. This was then averaged across trials, by participant. Trials in which participants did not make an anticipatory fixation to either card were excluded from analysis (30.5 \% of trials were excluded for this reason). All participants were required to contribute at least 3 trials (30 \%) to be included in the analysis. 2.2. Results To determine if 8-month-olds succeed on the DMR paradigm with redundant labels, we first conducted the same analyses carried out by Kaldy et al. (2016). We conducted two one-tailed, one-sample t-tests, comparing First Look and Proportion Looking Time to chance (.5), with the prediction that both measures will be significantly greater than chance. Greater than chance looking to the match card is evidence of success on the DMR. Findings demonstrated that neither First Look (t(18) = -1.48, p = .92, M = .45, SD = .15, 95 \% CI = [.39, Inf]), nor Proportion Looking (t(18) = -1.09, p = .85, M = .46, SD = .15, 95 \% CI = [.4, Inf]) measures were greater than chance. To determine if these participants out-perform the 8-month-old infants in the Kaldy et al. paradigm, we conducted a one-tailed, independent samples t-test comparing First Look and Proportion Looking Time across the Labels (current study) and No Labels (Kaldy et al., 2016) groups (Fig. 3). The goal of this analysis was to determine if labels provided a benefit to 8-month-olds, as compared to those who previously failed the task without labels. Findings demonstrated no significant differences between groups on First Look (t (30.95) = -.12, p = .91, 95 \% CI = [-.1, .09]), nor Proportion Looking (t(30.58) = -1.13, p = .27, 95 \% CI = [-.16, .04]) measures. Fig. 1. (A) Faces of Cards Used in Familiarization and Test, (B) Schematic of Familiarization Trial in Studies 1 and 2. Fig. 2. Schematic of Test Trial in Studies 1 and 2. A. Fitch et al. Infant Behavior and Development 64 (2021) 101617 5 3. Study 2 The goal of Study 2 was to determine what, if any, effect object labels have on infants’ WM representations when they are within their WM capacity. 10-month-olds have previously demonstrated successful memory for two object-location bindings without redundant labels. However, their prior performance on the DMR, while significantly above chance, averaged only 61.66 \% on First Look (Kaldy et al., 2016). This average suggests that they failed to make anticipatory looks to the match card nearly 40 \% of the time—this could potentially be improved if their object representations were more robust. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a DMR paradigm on two groups of 10-month-old infants: one who got redundant labels at exposure (identical to the Labels group in Study 1) and one that did not (a control group, conceptually akin to Kaldy et al., 2016). We hypothesized that if labels improve infants’ per- formance, the Label group would outperform the No Label group. If instead labels interfere with visual WM encoding, the No Label group would outperform the Label group. Finally, if labels have no effect, performance will be equal in the two groups (and the same as in Kaldy et al., 2016). 3.1. Method 3.1.1. Participants Participants were 34 healthy, full-term infants (14 females) ranging in age from 9;01–10;30 (M = 9;25; SD = 12.6 days). Nine additional infants participated, but were excluded for fussiness. Sample size was determined to be adequate using the same power analysis as in Study 1. Participants were assigned to one of two groups: Labels (n = 18) or No Labels (n = 16). Participants that were Fig. 3. Proportion correct performance based on First Look (A) and Proportion Looking Time (B) in Study 1. Dots represent individual participants. A. Fitch et al. Infant Behavior and Development 64 (2021) 101617 6 from dominantly (>70 \%) English-speaking households were randomly assigned to each group (n = 28). Participants that came from households that did not meet this threshold (n = 6) were assigned to the No Label group2 . Recruitment, consent, and payment procedures were identical to Study 1. 3.1.2. Stimuli, apparatus & procedure For the Label group, the stimuli, apparatus, and procedure were all identical to Study 1. For the No Label group, study features were also identical with the exception of the labeling phrases at encoding. The faces of the cards were presented in silence to this group. 3.1.3. Data analysis All data processing procedures and variables were identical to Study 1. As in Study 1, participants were required to contribute at least 3 valid trials (participants had to make at least one anticipatory fixation to either card in the memory set) to be included in the final sample. 3.2. Results To determine if 10-month-olds’ performance improves on the DMR paradigm with redundant labels, we compared anticipatory looking behavior between the two groups. We conducted two one-tailed, independent samples t-tests, comparing First Look and Proportion Looking Time across groups, with the hypothesis that the Label group would out-perform the No Label group on both measures (Fig. 4). Findings demonstrated that there were no significant differences between the two groups on either First Look (t (27.5) = -.68, p = .75, 95 \% CI = [-.16, Inf]) or Proportion Looking Time (t(31.23) = 1.15, p = .13, 95 \% CI = [-.033, Inf]). A post-hoc exploratory analysis demonstrated that the No Labels group was not significantly different from chance on First Look (t(15) = 1.70, p = 0.055, M = .56, SD = .14, 95 \% CI = [.5, Inf],). Because these results are close to the traditional p-value cutoff of significance and numerically similar to those found in Kaldy et al. (2016), we conducted a Bayesian one-tailed t-test to determine the meaningfulness of this result. Findings demonstrated that the alternative hypothesis is ~1.5 times more likely than the null hypothesis (BF10 = 1.54), which is considered weak or anecdotal evidence of the alternative hypothesis (Dienes, 2014). The No Labels group was not signifi- cantly different from chance on Proportion Looking Time (t(15) = -.33, p = .63, M = .49, SD = .18, 95 \% CI = [.41, Inf]. Finally, we examined the role of labels in performance across age groups (8-month-olds in Study 1 and 10-month-olds in Study 2). We used two-tailed independent-samples t-tests to compare First Look and Proportion Looking Time across the Labels groups from each study. We hypothesized that the 10-month-olds would out-perform the 8-month-olds on both measures. Findings demonstrated that this was not the case. The 10-month-olds were not significantly different from the 8-month-olds on measures of First Look (t(27.8) = -.95, p = .35, 95 \% CI = [-.20, .07]) nor Proportion Looking Time (t(33.87) = -1.74, p = .09, 95 \% CI = [-.2, .02]). 3.3. Discussion The overarching goal of these studies was to determine if redundant labels for familiar objects would facilitate WM performance in preverbal infants. In two studies, we demonstrated that this hypothesis was inconsistent with data from both 8- (Study 1) and 10- month-olds (Study 2). When presented with labels at encoding, infants in both age groups failed to make anticipatory looks to the match card above chance levels. Ten-month-olds who encoded the stimuli in silence did not perform significantly worse than 10- month-olds who were presented with concurrent labels, and surprisingly, they were not better than chance. These findings are not consistent with an account of labels benefiting WM in preverbal infants. We hypothesized that labels would benefit infant WM because prior work has demonstrated that redundant labels are beneficial to preverbal infants in other cognitive domains (e.g. Ferry et al., 2010). There are a number of reasons why this label superiority account may not have extended to our WM task. One possibility is that WM is an exception among cognitive skills, and infants at this age do not receive WM advantages when presented with labels. However, a narrower interpretation is possible, based on task demands. The Delayed Match Retrieval paradigm requires infants to not only remember the identities of the objects in the memory set, but also where each was located. Thus far, object labels have only been shown to benefit object-related processes (e.g. individuation, categorization, and recognition), but not object-location-binding processes. We did not use any spatial language, which has been shown to improve memory for object locations in 4-year-old children (Dessalegn & Landau, 2008, 2013). Thus, it is possible that labels could improve the encoding of objects themselves at this age, but not improve (or even interfere with) encoding of their locations. Indeed, prior research has suggested that communicative contexts engender memory biases for object identity whereas non-communicative contexts engender memory biases for object location (Yoon et al., 2008), although the replicability of this finding has recently been called into question (Silverstein et al., 2019). The primary theory competing with label superiority is auditory dominance (e.g. Robinson & Sloutsky, 2007; 2008), and our findings do not provide clear evidence for this account either. Whereas 10-month-olds who received redundant labels performed 2 This is akin to the procedure used in Kaldy et al. (2016) which, as it has been typical in infant cognition studies to date, did not exclude any participants on the basis of language exposure and did not analyze the effect of this factor (but see …
CATEGORIES
Economics Nursing Applied Sciences Psychology Science Management Computer Science Human Resource Management Accounting Information Systems English Anatomy Operations Management Sociology Literature Education Business & Finance Marketing Engineering Statistics Biology Political Science Reading History Financial markets Philosophy Mathematics Law Criminal Architecture and Design Government Social Science World history Chemistry Humanities Business Finance Writing Programming Telecommunications Engineering Geography Physics Spanish ach e. Embedded Entrepreneurship f. Three Social Entrepreneurship Models g. Social-Founder Identity h. Micros-enterprise Development Outcomes Subset 2. Indigenous Entrepreneurship Approaches (Outside of Canada) a. Indigenous Australian Entrepreneurs Exami Calculus (people influence of  others) processes that you perceived occurs in this specific Institution Select one of the forms of stratification highlighted (focus on inter the intersectionalities  of these three) to reflect and analyze the potential ways these ( American history Pharmacology Ancient history . Also Numerical analysis Environmental science Electrical Engineering Precalculus Physiology Civil Engineering Electronic Engineering ness Horizons Algebra Geology Physical chemistry nt When considering both O lassrooms Civil Probability ions Identify a specific consumer product that you or your family have used for quite some time. This might be a branded smartphone (if you have used several versions over the years) or the court to consider in its deliberations. Locard’s exchange principle argues that during the commission of a crime Chemical Engineering Ecology aragraphs (meaning 25 sentences or more). Your assignment may be more than 5 paragraphs but not less. INSTRUCTIONS:  To access the FNU Online Library for journals and articles you can go the FNU library link here:  https://www.fnu.edu/library/ In order to n that draws upon the theoretical reading to explain and contextualize the design choices. Be sure to directly quote or paraphrase the reading ce to the vaccine. Your campaign must educate and inform the audience on the benefits but also create for safe and open dialogue. A key metric of your campaign will be the direct increase in numbers.  Key outcomes: The approach that you take must be clear Mechanical Engineering Organic chemistry Geometry nment Topic You will need to pick one topic for your project (5 pts) Literature search You will need to perform a literature search for your topic Geophysics you been involved with a company doing a redesign of business processes Communication on Customer Relations. Discuss how two-way communication on social media channels impacts businesses both positively and negatively. Provide any personal examples from your experience od pressure and hypertension via a community-wide intervention that targets the problem across the lifespan (i.e. includes all ages). Develop a community-wide intervention to reduce elevated blood pressure and hypertension in the State of Alabama that in in body of the report Conclusions References (8 References Minimum) *** Words count = 2000 words. *** In-Text Citations and References using Harvard style. *** In Task section I’ve chose (Economic issues in overseas contracting)" Electromagnetism w or quality improvement; it was just all part of good nursing care.  The goal for quality improvement is to monitor patient outcomes using statistics for comparison to standards of care for different diseases e a 1 to 2 slide Microsoft PowerPoint presentation on the different models of case management.  Include speaker notes... .....Describe three different models of case management. visual representations of information. They can include numbers SSAY ame workbook for all 3 milestones. You do not need to download a new copy for Milestones 2 or 3. When you submit Milestone 3 pages): Provide a description of an existing intervention in Canada making the appropriate buying decisions in an ethical and professional manner. Topic: Purchasing and Technology You read about blockchain ledger technology. Now do some additional research out on the Internet and share your URL with the rest of the class be aware of which features their competitors are opting to include so the product development teams can design similar or enhanced features to attract more of the market. The more unique low (The Top Health Industry Trends to Watch in 2015) to assist you with this discussion.         https://youtu.be/fRym_jyuBc0 Next year the $2.8 trillion U.S. healthcare industry will   finally begin to look and feel more like the rest of the business wo evidence-based primary care curriculum. Throughout your nurse practitioner program Vignette Understanding Gender Fluidity Providing Inclusive Quality Care Affirming Clinical Encounters Conclusion References Nurse Practitioner Knowledge Mechanics and word limit is unit as a guide only. The assessment may be re-attempted on two further occasions (maximum three attempts in total). All assessments must be resubmitted 3 days within receiving your unsatisfactory grade. You must clearly indicate “Re-su Trigonometry Article writing Other 5. June 29 After the components sending to the manufacturing house 1. In 1972 the Furman v. Georgia case resulted in a decision that would put action into motion. Furman was originally sentenced to death because of a murder he committed in Georgia but the court debated whether or not this was a violation of his 8th amend One of the first conflicts that would need to be investigated would be whether the human service professional followed the responsibility to client ethical standard.  While developing a relationship with client it is important to clarify that if danger or Ethical behavior is a critical topic in the workplace because the impact of it can make or break a business No matter which type of health care organization With a direct sale During the pandemic Computers are being used to monitor the spread of outbreaks in different areas of the world and with this record 3. Furman v. Georgia is a U.S Supreme Court case that resolves around the Eighth Amendments ban on cruel and unsual punishment in death penalty cases. The Furman v. Georgia case was based on Furman being convicted of murder in Georgia. Furman was caught i One major ethical conflict that may arise in my investigation is the Responsibility to Client in both Standard 3 and Standard 4 of the Ethical Standards for Human Service Professionals (2015).  Making sure we do not disclose information without consent ev 4. Identify two examples of real world problems that you have observed in your personal Summary & Evaluation: Reference & 188. Academic Search Ultimate Ethics We can mention at least one example of how the violation of ethical standards can be prevented. Many organizations promote ethical self-regulation by creating moral codes to help direct their business activities *DDB is used for the first three years For example The inbound logistics for William Instrument refer to purchase components from various electronic firms. During the purchase process William need to consider the quality and price of the components. In this case 4. A U.S. Supreme Court case known as Furman v. Georgia (1972) is a landmark case that involved Eighth Amendment’s ban of unusual and cruel punishment in death penalty cases (Furman v. Georgia (1972) With covid coming into place In my opinion with Not necessarily all home buyers are the same! When you choose to work with we buy ugly houses Baltimore & nationwide USA The ability to view ourselves from an unbiased perspective allows us to critically assess our personal strengths and weaknesses. This is an important step in the process of finding the right resources for our personal learning style. Ego and pride can be · By Day 1 of this week While you must form your answers to the questions below from our assigned reading material CliftonLarsonAllen LLP (2013) 5 The family dynamic is awkward at first since the most outgoing and straight forward person in the family in Linda Urien The most important benefit of my statistical analysis would be the accuracy with which I interpret the data. The greatest obstacle From a similar but larger point of view 4 In order to get the entire family to come back for another session I would suggest coming in on a day the restaurant is not open When seeking to identify a patient’s health condition After viewing the you tube videos on prayer Your paper must be at least two pages in length (not counting the title and reference pages) The word assimilate is negative to me. I believe everyone should learn about a country that they are going to live in. It doesnt mean that they have to believe that everything in America is better than where they came from. It means that they care enough Data collection Single Subject Chris is a social worker in a geriatric case management program located in a midsize Northeastern town. She has an MSW and is part of a team of case managers that likes to continuously improve on its practice. The team is currently using an I would start off with Linda on repeating her options for the child and going over what she is feeling with each option.  I would want to find out what she is afraid of.  I would avoid asking her any “why” questions because I want her to be in the here an Summarize the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psychological research (Comp 2.1) 25.0\% Summarization of the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psych Identify the type of research used in a chosen study Compose a 1 Optics effect relationship becomes more difficult—as the researcher cannot enact total control of another person even in an experimental environment. Social workers serve clients in highly complex real-world environments. Clients often implement recommended inte I think knowing more about you will allow you to be able to choose the right resources Be 4 pages in length soft MB-920 dumps review and documentation and high-quality listing pdf MB-920 braindumps also recommended and approved by Microsoft experts. The practical test g One thing you will need to do in college is learn how to find and use references. References support your ideas. College-level work must be supported by research. You are expected to do that for this paper. You will research Elaborate on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study 20.0\% Elaboration on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study is missing. Elaboration on any potenti 3 The first thing I would do in the family’s first session is develop a genogram of the family to get an idea of all the individuals who play a major role in Linda’s life. After establishing where each member is in relation to the family A Health in All Policies approach Note: The requirements outlined below correspond to the grading criteria in the scoring guide. At a minimum Chen Read Connecting Communities and Complexity: A Case Study in Creating the Conditions for Transformational Change Read Reflections on Cultural Humility Read A Basic Guide to ABCD Community Organizing Use the bolded black section and sub-section titles below to organize your paper. For each section Losinski forwarded the article on a priority basis to Mary Scott Losinksi wanted details on use of the ED at CGH. He asked the administrative resident