Readings - English
BRIEF REPORT
THE USE OF ERRORLESS LEARNING
PROCEDURES IN TEACHING PEOPLE
WITH A LEARNING DISABILITY:
A CRITICAL REVIEW
Robert S.P. Jones a n d C B . Eayrs
University College of North Wales, Bangor, Gwynedd, LL57 2DG
Abstract The use of procedures which minimise the making of errors
is a popular method of teaching skills to people with learning disability.
The origin of this approach can be traced t o two distinct sources: the
work of B.F. Skinner on programmed learning, and the work of H.S.
Terrace on discrimination learning. This early work is reviewed and
research findings which highlight the negative side affects of an
‘errorless’ approach are discussed. The role of prompting, attention,
reinforcement and generalisation is outlined. Recommendations for the
development of teaching programmes are made.
Introduction
One of the most fundamental aspects of skill acquisition involves the
development of stimulus control. It can be argued that the ability of an
organism to discriminate between stimuli with which it is presented
represents the most basic foundation of learning. The acquisition of an
operant discrimination has been defined as the process whereby an
organism comes to respond more frequently to a stimulus correlated with
reinforcement, S + , than to a stimulus associated with non-reinforcement,
S - (Terrace, 1963a).
Historically, the learning of an operant discrimination involved the
differential reinforcement of responses which occurred in the presence of the
S + and the extinction of responses which occurred in the presence of S - .
This procedure is most commonly known as ‘trial-and-error’ learning with
any response in the presence of S- being regarded as an error. The making of
errors in acquiring a discrimination was not only therefore extremely
common in the development of stimulus control but it was frequently
assumed that the making of errors was an essential occurrence if an organism
was to master a discrimination (e.g. Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950).
09.52-9608/92/02 0204-9 $01.80/0
MENTAL HANDICAP RESEARCH
204
0 1992 R . Jones & C. Eayrs
Vo1.5, No.2, 1992
USING ERRORLESS LEARNING PROCEDURES 205
In applied work with people with learning disabilities, discrimination
learning using trial-and-error methods was limited by a number of factors
which involved the making of mistakes or errors. It was felt that for some
people with a learning disability the demands of learning by trial and error
could provoke problem behaviours (Carr, Newsom & Binkoff, 1980; Weeks
& Gaylord-Ross, 1981). Problems such as ‘apathy, aggression, self-injury,
negativism and tantrumming’ have been cited as direct consequences of the
trial-and-error learning approach (Touchette & Howard, 1984; Hamblin et
a/. ,197 1).
In the early 1960s H.S. Terrace conducted a series of now famous animal
experiments which suggested that not only was the making of errors
unnecessary in the development of learning but that learning was actually
facilitated if no errors were made during the acquisition of the
discrimination (Terrace, 1963a, 1963b,; 1964; 1966). Essentially Terrace’s
early work involved the use of trial and error procedure designed to teach
pigeons to peck a key illuminated with a red light but not to peck a key
illuminated with a green light. When the pigeons pecked the red stimulus
( S + ) they received a food pellet and when they pecked the green stimulus
(S - ), nothing happened. Both keys were present at the same illumination
during the experiment. A second group of pigeons began the experiment
with the green key dark and had the light behind it slowly ‘faded in’ during
the training condition. By the end of the experiment the second group also
had two illuminated keys of equal intensity present. The second group made
very few responses to the green light. Indeed it was as if they hadn’t noticed
its presence. The stimulus fading procedure (resulting in few, if any,
responses being made to the S - ) was called errorless learning.
Although Terrace’s work has not led to major changes in the theoretical
explanations of discrimination learning (Robinson & Storm, 1978), this
early work stimulated subsequent research which has had a major effect on
strategies for teaching people with learning disabilities.
In the years immediately following Terrace’s early work, errorless
procedures were employed in teaching simple discriminations to children
with learning disabilities (e.g. Sidman & Stoddard, 1966, 1967; Touchette,
1968). This work, supported Terrace’s claim that discrimination learning
was facilitated by the use of errorless learning techniques. Perhaps the
major impact of this work, however, was the finding that children with
learning disabilities could be taught by errorless teaching methods in cases
where such discriminations could not be acquired by traditional trial-and-
error techniques (Walsh, 1985).
Another influence on ‘errorless’ teaching technology came from
Skinner’s pioneering research on applied behaviour analysis and
206 MENTAL HANDICAP RESEARCH
programmed learning (Skinner, 1968). This research suggested that errors
could be avoided by breaking tasks down into small steps, by the use of
shaping through successive approximations and by the optimal spacing of
reinforcers.
As a result of these influences, the errorless learning approach was
quickly extended beyond basic discrimination training to encompass a
complete technology of teaching covering areas far removed from Terrace’s
original work. An emphasis on an ‘errorless’ approach to teaching people
with learning disabilities became increasingly popular and to this day many
books and training manuals owe their origin, overtly or covertly, to this
approach (Best, 1987; Donnellan, La Vigna, Negri-Shoultz & Fassbender,
1988; Foxen & McBrien, 1981; La Vigna & Donnellan, 1986; Smith, 1990;
Zarkowska & Clements, 1988)
Although there can be little doubt that the influence of errorless learning
techniques on the training of people with learning disabilities has been of
great benefit, a number of problems exist with this approach.
Difficulties with Errorless Learning
Narrowing of attention
Conclusions from a number of studies have suggested that as early as
1966 some researchers were questioning the efficacy of errorless techniques
in certain situations (cf. Walsh, 1985).
The major conclusions from those studies suggest that fading techniques
may confine the person’s attention to very narrow attributes of the stimulus
associated with reinforcement (S + ). Richell, for example, questioned the
adaptability of a person who had been trained through errorless procedures
and particularly the ability to cope in new situations where errors would
occur (Richell, 1966). Bijou & Baer (1966) also questioned the ability to
transfer skills to other settings. Both of these researchers made these
suggestions on theoretical grounds.
In 1968, however, Gollin & Savoy conducted a series of experiments
which provided experimental evidence for the assertion that in some
situations errorless learning may result in a greater number of mistakes
compared to trial and error learning once transfer of learning to a new
situation is involved. Gollin & Savoy (1968) divided a sample of 52 children
into two groups. Both groups were taught a discrimination task followed
by a conditional discrimination task (where the nature of the response is
dependent on the nature of the discriminative stimulus). One group was
taught by a trial-and-error procedure and one by using an errorless fading
USING ERRORLESS LEARNING PROCEDURES 207
technique. Although more children who were trained by the fading
procedure performed without errors during training, more children in the
trial-and-error group solved the conditional discrimination problem. Thus,
it appeared that for simple discriminations requiring a single response to a
stimulus, errorless methods resulted in fewer errors. When the task became
more complicated, however, with the use of a conditional discrimination
task then the errorless group did not perform as well.
Similar findings have been reported by a number of authors, (e.g. Wolfe
& Cuvo, 1978; Walsh, 1985). Walsh (1985) compared errorless and trial-
and-error procedures o n a conditional discrimination test. The results of
Walsh’s (1985) study were similar to those of Gollin & Savoy (1968). Again
errorless learning techniques worked very well when the task was a very
simple one requiring only a simple response. When the task became more
complicated and necessitated paying attention to more than one stimulus,
then errorless learning proved to be an inferior teaching technique when
compared with trial-and-error learning. Walsh concluded that ‘under
certain conditions fading techniques are not able to provide optimal
conditions for learning a given task’ (Walsh, 1985:36).
It appears therefore, that access to a combination of both S + and S -
throughout training is important in all but the most simple discrimination
tasks.
New responses
In situations where a completely new response was required (as opposed
to strengthening an existing behaviour) it was assumed that the optimum
mechanism for generating the new response was the prompting of that
response and the gradual fading of the prompt as the behaviour became
established. No direct experimental evidence was obtained which indicated
that this method was, in fact, the optimum means of generating new
responses. Rather, this appears to have been an intuitive ‘common sense’
decision and it is possible that the emphasis placed on avoiding errors in
accounting-for successful teaching, and the consequent generalisation of the
methodology, might be misplaced.
Although the work of Skinner (1968) is perhaps the most relevant to the
operant teaching of new skills, the term ‘errorless’ has been traditionally
applied to the work of Terrace rather than Skinner. The danger here is that
this confusion could lead to the assumption that the methodology which is
appropriate for teaching pigeons to peck red rather than green illuminated
keys will also be appropriate for teaching self-care skills to people with a
learning disability. A closer examination of the procedures used, however,
MENTAL HANDICAP RESEARCH
reveal a number of factors, apart from ‘errorlessness’ which could account
for success or failure.
In the typical two choice discrimination procedure, the animal or person
typically makes a simple ‘pointing’ response. This may either be a pecking
response (Terrace, 1963a) or a gestural indication (Cullen, 1978). The
prompt to respond is the actual stimulus which is designed to gain control
over the behaviour once learning has taken place. Furthermore, at some
level a choice is being made from the outset. This can be conceived of as
a trial-and-error scenario with the dice loaded towards success.
When this procedure is compared with the use of ‘errorless’ procedures
to teach self-help skills to people with learning disability a number of
differences become apparent. Firstly, the motor behaviour is frequently far
more complex than simply ‘pointing’ (e.g. brushing teeth, feeding with a
spoon). Secondly, the form that the prompting takes is different. The
avoidance of errors is achieved by the physical prompting of movement by
the trainer. The person has no choice about what he/she does. At its
extreme, the person may be so passive in the procedure that he/she may
initially be little more than an extension of the trainer’s own musculature.
Thirdly, the discriminative stimuli during training are not always the same
as those which are t o gain final control at the end of the training procedure.
For example, the final discriminative stimulus for handwashing would
normally be the presence of dirt. During training, however, it is the presence
of the trainer, and the use of a prompting procedure which acts as a
discriminative stimulus. It is these aspects of the training procedure which
are so difficult to ‘fade out’. Fourthly, in Terrace’s errorless discrimination
learning procedure the S + remained constant throughout whilst S - was
‘faded in’. In the self-help skills teaching situation, however, S +
continually changes as prompts are ‘faded out’. Thus, it may be an
oversimplification to simply compare errorless versus trial-and-error
training unless these other aspects of the environment are controlled.
Generalisation
A further disadvantage with the errorless approach seems to lie in the
area of generalisation. As was mentioned earlier, many errorlessly trained
discriminations require a prompting component. In such cases, the person’s
ability to generalise a learned skill is wholly dependent on the success with
which the prompting component can be gradually withdrawn or faded out.
Unfortunately, ‘it is often the case that students who respond appropriately
when prompted founder when the prompt is removed’. (Touchette &
Howard, 1984:175). In practice, the fading of prompts is a
USING ERRORLESS LEARNING PROCEDURES 209
very difficult part of teaching. Premature removal of prompts can lead to
persistent incorrect response patterns which preclude acquisition of the
target repertoire (Sidman & Stoddard, 1966; Touchette, 1968). If, on the
other hand, prompts are presented for an unnecessarily extended period
then the person may become dependent on the prompt. Either of these
extremes will preclude successful generalisation of learning to novel
situations. It is, nevertheless, all too common t o find that a person with a
learning disability can only display a learned skill with the aid of a prompt,
despite many unsuccessful attempts to fade out prompting. As Touchette
& Howard (1984) have said ‘. . . research has not yet resolved the question
of how to produce a successful transfer from prompted to unprompted
responding’ (p. 175).
Reinforcement
One of the major advantages of an errorless approach lies in the high
frequency of reinforcement available to the person. In teaching people with
a learning disability many authors (e.g. Foxen & McBrien, 1981), have
advocated that each prompted response should be followed by
reinforcement. This continuous reinforcement (CRF) schedule, however, is
unlikely to lead to spontaneous generalisation to new situations. There
exists a wide variety of research data which suggests that once continuous
reinforcement is withdrawn, the acquired behaviour quickly extinguishes
(Kazdin & Polster, 1973; Koegel & Rincover, 1977; Kazdin, 1984).
Although it has been advocated that, as soon as a behaviour is acquired on
a CRF schedule, reinforcement should be switched to an intermittent
schedule, there have been a number of difficulties with this procedure
(Tierney & Smith 1988; Dehn, 1969). Dehn (1969) found that the
smoothness of transfer from continuous to intermittent schedules is a key
factor in the success of the procedure. Hamblin et al. (1971) in discussing
this work, concluded that ‘after acquisition during the transition from
continuous to intermittent reinforcement, negative behaviour is increased in
proportion t o the haste with which the transition is made’ (p. 153).
Research carried out with children with learning disabilities (Tierney &
Smith, 1988), demonstrated that responding on an intermittent schedule
during the maintenance phase of a training programme was more likely if
initial response acquisition was programmed using a partial schedule than
if response acquisition was initially programmed using a CRF schedule and
then switched t o an intermittent schedule (Tierney & Smith, 1988). This
suggests that even from the beginnings of teaching a discrimination, not all
correct responses should be reinforced if optimal response generalisation is
210 MENTAL HANDICAP RESEARCH
to be attained. Clearly, this would mean that one of the major aspects asso-
ciated with an errorless approach (each response is correct and leads to rein-
forcement) could not occur. A dilemma between speed of initial response
acquisition and optimum generalisation of responding could thus be
engendered.
A further issue with regard to reinforcement and subsequent
generalisation involves the timing of reinforcement administration. It is
frequently assumed that the time delay between responding and contingent
reinforcement should be as short as possible and this is usually advocated
in teaching materials (e.g. Foxen & McBrien, 1981). In the natural
environment, however, there is often a delay between responding and
reinforcement and it has been suggested that such a delay may actually
facilitate generalisation (Kazdin, 1982).
Summary
In summary, although errorless learning has undoubtedly provided a
technology of teaching which has led to significant advances in the
education of people with a learning disability, a number of difficulties
remain with this approach. Firstly, fading techniques appear to result in a
narrowing of attention which may inhibit the subsequent learning of more
complex behaviours. Secondly, where errorless prompting procedures are
used, difficulties may arise in the removal of the prompts due to an
overdependence on their availability. Thirdly, and perhaps most
importantly, there are difficulties with the subsequent maintainence and
generalisation of behaviours acquired through errorless methods.
Conclusions
While the literature clearly points out that a learned discrimination can
be achieved and maintained by specifying the setting conditions (prompts)
and locating the maintaining events in the natural environment, it is
suggested that prompting should be as infrequent as possible and that where
possible the learner should have access to a wide range of stimuli in addition
to the S + . It can be argued that errorless learning methods should only by
used in situations where trial-and-error methods have proven unsuccessful.
The preferred treatment approach should be the shaping of the desired
response, by successive approximations in the natural environment. Thus,
an attempt should be made to teach using trial-and-error methods in the
first instance before fading procedures are employed.
USING ERRORLESS LEARNING PROCEDURES 21 1
Once the decision to use errorless methods has been made, specific
strategies need to be incorporated to take account of prompting, attention,
reinforcement and generalisation.
Preventing errors at all costs does not seem to merit the same emphasis
that Terrace’s early work might suggest. In particular, it is not
recommended that overprompting is used t o avoid making mistakes. In
terms of subsequent generalisation, it is probably more advantageous for
a client to make a mistake in mastering a discrimination than to become
dependent on prompting. Each individual, whatever his o r her degree of
learning difficulty, brings a unique constellation of skills, experience, and
talent t o any new learning situation. It is important to adapt all teaching
t o be as flexible and individualistic as possible and not to attempt to fit the
learner into a predetermined system of training o r instruction.
References
Best, A.B. (1987) Steps to Independence: Practical Guidance on Teaching People
with Mental and Sensory Handicaps. Kidderminster: BIMH Publications.
Bijou, S.W. and Baer, D.M. (1966) Operant methods in child behavior and
development. In W.K. Honig (ed) Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and
Application. New Y ork : Appleton-Centur y-Cro fts .
Carr, T., Newsom, C. and Binkoff, J. (1980) Escape as a factor in the aggressive
behavior of two retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 13,
Cullen, C.N. (1978) Errorless learning with the retarded. Journal of Practical
Approaches to Developmental Handicap 2, 21 -4.
Dehn, J. (1969) An Investigation of the Development and Maintenance of the
Negative Behavior of Autistic Children. Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation,
Washington University (cited in Hamblin et al., 1971).
Donnellan, A.M., LaVigna, G.W., Negri-Shoultz, N. and Fassbender, L.L. (1988)
Progress Without Punishment: Effective Approaches for Learners with
Behavior Problems. New York: Teachers College Press.
Foxen, T. and McBrien, J. (1981) Training Staff in Behavioral Methods: Trainee
Workbook. Manchester:Manchester University Press.
Collin, E.S. and Savoy, P. (1968) Fading procedures and conditional discrimination
in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 1 1, 443 - 5 1.
Hamblin, E.L., Buckholdt, D., Ferritor, D., Kozloff, M. and Blackwell, L. (1971)
The Humanization Process: A Social, Behavioral Analysis of Children’s
Problems. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
Kazdin, A.E. (1982) Single-Case Research Designs: Methods for Clinical and
Applied Settings. New York: Oxford University Press.
-( 1984) Behavior Modification in Applied Settings. Homewood: Dorsey Press.
Kazdin, A.E. and Polster, R. (1973) Intermittent token reinforcement and response
Keller, F.S. and Schoenfield, W.N. (1950) Principles of Psychology. New York:
101 - 17.
maintenance in extinction. Behavior Therapy 4, 386-91.
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
212 MENTAL HANDICAP RESEARCH
Koegel, R.L. and Rincover, A. (1977) Research on the difference between
generalisation and maintenance in extra-therapy responding. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis 10, 1 - 12.
LaVigna, G.W. and Donnellan, A.M. (1986) Alternatives to Punishment: Solving
Behavior Problems with Non-aversive Strategies. New York: Irvington.
Richell, M. (1966) L’apprentissage sans erreurs. L’Annee Psychologique 2, 535-43
(cited in Walsh, 1985).
Robinson, P.W. and Storm, R.H. (1978) Effects of error and errorless
discrimination acquisition on reversal learning. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior 29, 517-25.
Sidman, M., and Stoddard, C.T. (1966) Programming perception and learning for
retarded children. In N.R. Ellis (ed.) International Review of Research in
Mental Retardation Vol. 11. New York: Academic Press.
- (1967) The effectiveness of fading in programming a stimultaneous form
discrimination for retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior 20, 3- 15.
Skinner, B.F. (1968) The Technology of Teaching. New York: Meredith
Corporation.
Smith, M.D. (1990) Autism and Life in the Community: Successful Interventions
for Behavioral Challenges. Baltimore: Paul H . Brooks.
Terrace, H . S. (1963a) Discrimination learning with and without errors. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 4, 1-27.
- (1963b) Errorless transfers of a discrimination across two continua. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 6, 223-32.
- (1964) Wavelength generalisation after discrimination learning with and
without errors. Science 114, 78-80.
- (1966) Stimulus control. In W.K. Honig (ed.) Operant Behavior. Areas of
Research and Application (pp 271 - 344). New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts.
Tierney, K.J. and Smith, H.V. (1988) The effects of various combinations of
continuous and partial reinforcement schedules on response persistence in
mentally handicapped children. Behavioral Psychotherapy 16, 23 - 37.
Touchette, P.E. (1968) The effects of graduated stimulus change on the acquisition
of a simple discrimination in severely retarded boys. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11, 39-48.
Touchette, P.E. and Howard, J.S. (1984) Errorless learning: Reinforcement
contingencies and stimulus control transfer in delayed prompting. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis 17, 175-88.
Walsh, P.G. (1985) Teaching colour discrimination t o the mentally handicapped.
The Irish Journal of Psychology 7, 36-49.
Weeks, M. and Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981) Task difficulty and abberant behavior in
severely handicapped students. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 14,
449-3.
Wolfe, V. F. and Cuvo, A.J. (1978) Effects of within-stimulus and extra stimulus
prompting on letter discrimination by mentally retarded persons. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency 83, 297-303.
Zarkowska, E. and Clements, J. (1988) Problem Behaviour in People with Severe
Learning Disabilities: A Practical Guide to A Constructional Approach.
London: Croom Helm.
JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR
TWO "SYNTHETIC SOCIAL RELATIONS"
B. F. SKINNER
HARVARD UNIVERSITY
About 10 years ago, two demonstration ex-
periments were designed for a General Educa-
tion course in Human Behavior at Harvard.
They were briefly described in an illustrated
weekly and are occasionally referred to in the
psychological literature. It seems advisable to
publish a somewhat more explicit account.
THE "PING-PONG" PLAYING PIGEONS
There were several versions of this ap-
paratus, in one of which a motor-driven device
returned the ping-pong ball to the playing
surface so that the apparatus ran without at-
tention. In a less mechanized version, the
"ping-pong" table was approximately 8 in.
wide, 16 in. long, and 8 in. high (Fig. 1). A
pigeon standing at one end could conveniently
peck a ball as it arrived at the edge of the
table. If the ball rolled off the edge, it fell into
a trough and tripped a switch which operated
a food dispenser under the opposite edge and
thus reinforced the pigeon which "won the
point." Light metal rails prevented the ball
from falling off the sides of the table. The
surface was slightly canted, sloping from a
center line toward each edge so that the ball
would not stop on it. Wire barriers prevented
the pigeons from jumping up on the table but
did not interfere with play.
In the finished performance, the demon-
strator would start a ball near the middle of
the table. It rolled to one edge and the pigeon
on that side pecked it, driving it back across
the table. At the other edge it was pecked by
the other pigeon and thus returned. The
pigeons usually watched the course of the ball
as it crossed the table, and maneuvered into
position to meet the return. They developed
considerable skill in sending the ball straight
across. Moving pictures show rallies of as many
as five or six shots before a point was made.
There is no evidence, however, that either
pigeon reached the stage of placing or chang-
Fig. 1. Two pigeons "playing ping-pong."
531
VOLUME 5, NUMBER 4 OCTOBER, 1962
B. F. SKINNER
ing the pace of its shots so that the opponent
would miss.
Conditioning was begun with one pigeon at
a time. A standard table tennis ball was
fastened at the edge of the table, and a hungry
pigeon was reinforced with food when it
pecked it. At this stage the ball was not a
powerful controlling stimulus; when it was
moved to a different part of the edge, the
pigeons often pecked the air where it had
been. Eventually, however, they pecked the
ball regardless of its position. The ball was
then made free to roll away from the pigeon
when struck. A mechanical reinforcing system
was set up in which the ball, rolling up a slight
grade, struck a cross-bar operating the food
dispenser. The distance to the bar was
gradually increased. If the ball failed to reach
the bar, it rolled back and came to rest against
a raised molding along the edge. The molding
was later removed.
As the distance between the pigeon and the
reinforcing bar was increased, reinforcement
was more and more delayed, and the behavior
occasionally suffered. Eventually, however,
mediating behavior arose to bridge the tem-
poral gap. Even so, in the final game, in which
two pigeons participated, the delay between
striking the ball and the successful outcome of
getting the ball past the opponent was occa-
sionally troublesome. A deteriorating perform-
ance could be rescued by reinforcing a pigeon
with a hand-switch at the moment it struck the
ball. Eventually the behavior was sustained
not only for rallies of several shots at a time
but for a full "game."
The demonstration offers a convenient ex-
ample of competition. One bird is reinforced
at the expense of another. If one is repeatedly
successful, the other suffers extinction ("dis-
couragement"). It was possible to maintain a
reasonable balance in successful play by lower-
ing the weight of the relatively unsuccessful
bird or raising that of the successful, the prin-
cipal effect being to sustain attention rather
than alter accuracy or power.
COOPERATING PIGEONS
Two pigeons in adjacent compartments
were separated by a pane of glass. Three red
buttons were arranged in a vertical row on
each side of the glass, as shown in Fig. 2. The
buttons were approximately 10 in., 7-1/2 in.,
and 5 in. from the floor, respectively. By peck-
ing a button the pigeon closed a switch. In the
final performance, both pigeons were rein-
forced with food (Fig. 2, below) when they
pecked a corresponding pair of buttons so
nearly simultaneously that the brief closures
of the circuits (each lasting perhaps a tenth of
a second) overlapped. At any given time, how-
Fig. 2. Above: Two pigeons cooperating by pecking
corresponding buttons at the same moment. Below:
Pigeons eating from food dispensers.
ever, only one pair of buttons was operative,
and the effective pair was scheduled in a
roughly random way.
It was necessary for the pigeons to cooperate
in two tasks: (1) discovering the effective pair
and (2) pecking both buttons at the same time.
In general, no pattern of exploration could be
observed. The pigeons tested all three pairs of
buttons in what was evidentally an unsystem-
atic way. In general, there was a division of
"SYNTHETIC SOCIAL RELATIONS"
labor with respect to the two tasks. One pigeon
(the "leader") explored-that is, it struck the
three buttons in some order. A similar per-
formance could have been generated in one
pigeon alone in the apparatus by requiring
simply that a given one of three buttons be
struck. The other pigeon (the "follower")
struck the button opposite that being struck
by the leader. Similar behavior could have
been generated in one pigeon alone in the
apparatus if one button after another had
been marked by a discriminative stimulus.
A well-marked leader-follower relation
could be established or reversed by altering
the relative level of food deprivation, the more
deprived bird assuming the position of leader
by moving more alertly to the buttons. How-
ever, even a decisive leader was probably to
some extent following. A deprived pigeon
would usually "wait to be followed" by one
less deprived before exploring the buttons
vigorously. Under levels of deprivation at
which both birds responded quickly and with-
out interruption, performance became so per-
fect that it gave the impression of one pigeon
seen in a mirror.
The performance was established by con-
ditioning each bird separately to peck the
three buttons, reinforcement being roughly
randomized. When sustained behavior oc-
curred on all three buttons, two birds could be
put in the adjacent spaces for the first time.
The presence of another bird temporarily dis-
turbed the performance, but both birds
eventually began to respond to the buttons.
At this stage responses to corresponding but-
tons within, say, half a second of each other
would trigger both food-dispensers. These
contingencies sufficed to build cooperative be-
havior without further attention. The visual
stimulation supplied by one pigeon pecking
on a button became a discriminative stimulus
controlling a response to the corresponding
button on the part of the other.
Prolonged exposure to these conditions
made pigeons strongly imitative in other re-
spects. They would often drink from glasses
of water in the compartments at the same time,
for example. The extent to which their be-
haviors were mutually controlled was infor-
mally demonstrated when the experiment was
shown to a group of biologists, one of whom
suggested putting the birds in the opposite
compartments. The birds immediately lined
up alongside the glass plate, facing away from
the buttons. They thus assumed their pre-
viously effective positions relative to each
other, but were now facing the audience
through the transparent front wall of the ap-
paratus. Though no buttons were available,
they immediately began to cooperate in ex-
ploring a corresponding area, bobbing up and
down in a perfect mirror-image pattern under
the control of each other's behavior. Possibly
because the leader-follower relation had fre-
quently been shifted, each bird was evidently
largely controlled by the behavior of the other.
Supported by NSF Grant G18167.
533
CATEGORIES
Economics
Nursing
Applied Sciences
Psychology
Science
Management
Computer Science
Human Resource Management
Accounting
Information Systems
English
Anatomy
Operations Management
Sociology
Literature
Education
Business & Finance
Marketing
Engineering
Statistics
Biology
Political Science
Reading
History
Financial markets
Philosophy
Mathematics
Law
Criminal
Architecture and Design
Government
Social Science
World history
Chemistry
Humanities
Business Finance
Writing
Programming
Telecommunications Engineering
Geography
Physics
Spanish
ach
e. Embedded Entrepreneurship
f. Three Social Entrepreneurship Models
g. Social-Founder Identity
h. Micros-enterprise Development
Outcomes
Subset 2. Indigenous Entrepreneurship Approaches (Outside of Canada)
a. Indigenous Australian Entrepreneurs Exami
Calculus
(people influence of
others) processes that you perceived occurs in this specific Institution Select one of the forms of stratification highlighted (focus on inter the intersectionalities
of these three) to reflect and analyze the potential ways these (
American history
Pharmacology
Ancient history
. Also
Numerical analysis
Environmental science
Electrical Engineering
Precalculus
Physiology
Civil Engineering
Electronic Engineering
ness Horizons
Algebra
Geology
Physical chemistry
nt
When considering both O
lassrooms
Civil
Probability
ions
Identify a specific consumer product that you or your family have used for quite some time. This might be a branded smartphone (if you have used several versions over the years)
or the court to consider in its deliberations. Locard’s exchange principle argues that during the commission of a crime
Chemical Engineering
Ecology
aragraphs (meaning 25 sentences or more). Your assignment may be more than 5 paragraphs but not less.
INSTRUCTIONS:
To access the FNU Online Library for journals and articles you can go the FNU library link here:
https://www.fnu.edu/library/
In order to
n that draws upon the theoretical reading to explain and contextualize the design choices. Be sure to directly quote or paraphrase the reading
ce to the vaccine. Your campaign must educate and inform the audience on the benefits but also create for safe and open dialogue. A key metric of your campaign will be the direct increase in numbers.
Key outcomes: The approach that you take must be clear
Mechanical Engineering
Organic chemistry
Geometry
nment
Topic
You will need to pick one topic for your project (5 pts)
Literature search
You will need to perform a literature search for your topic
Geophysics
you been involved with a company doing a redesign of business processes
Communication on Customer Relations. Discuss how two-way communication on social media channels impacts businesses both positively and negatively. Provide any personal examples from your experience
od pressure and hypertension via a community-wide intervention that targets the problem across the lifespan (i.e. includes all ages).
Develop a community-wide intervention to reduce elevated blood pressure and hypertension in the State of Alabama that in
in body of the report
Conclusions
References (8 References Minimum)
*** Words count = 2000 words.
*** In-Text Citations and References using Harvard style.
*** In Task section I’ve chose (Economic issues in overseas contracting)"
Electromagnetism
w or quality improvement; it was just all part of good nursing care. The goal for quality improvement is to monitor patient outcomes using statistics for comparison to standards of care for different diseases
e a 1 to 2 slide Microsoft PowerPoint presentation on the different models of case management. Include speaker notes... .....Describe three different models of case management.
visual representations of information. They can include numbers
SSAY
ame workbook for all 3 milestones. You do not need to download a new copy for Milestones 2 or 3. When you submit Milestone 3
pages):
Provide a description of an existing intervention in Canada
making the appropriate buying decisions in an ethical and professional manner.
Topic: Purchasing and Technology
You read about blockchain ledger technology. Now do some additional research out on the Internet and share your URL with the rest of the class
be aware of which features their competitors are opting to include so the product development teams can design similar or enhanced features to attract more of the market. The more unique
low (The Top Health Industry Trends to Watch in 2015) to assist you with this discussion.
https://youtu.be/fRym_jyuBc0
Next year the $2.8 trillion U.S. healthcare industry will finally begin to look and feel more like the rest of the business wo
evidence-based primary care curriculum. Throughout your nurse practitioner program
Vignette
Understanding Gender Fluidity
Providing Inclusive Quality Care
Affirming Clinical Encounters
Conclusion
References
Nurse Practitioner Knowledge
Mechanics
and word limit is unit as a guide only.
The assessment may be re-attempted on two further occasions (maximum three attempts in total). All assessments must be resubmitted 3 days within receiving your unsatisfactory grade. You must clearly indicate “Re-su
Trigonometry
Article writing
Other
5. June 29
After the components sending to the manufacturing house
1. In 1972 the Furman v. Georgia case resulted in a decision that would put action into motion. Furman was originally sentenced to death because of a murder he committed in Georgia but the court debated whether or not this was a violation of his 8th amend
One of the first conflicts that would need to be investigated would be whether the human service professional followed the responsibility to client ethical standard. While developing a relationship with client it is important to clarify that if danger or
Ethical behavior is a critical topic in the workplace because the impact of it can make or break a business
No matter which type of health care organization
With a direct sale
During the pandemic
Computers are being used to monitor the spread of outbreaks in different areas of the world and with this record
3. Furman v. Georgia is a U.S Supreme Court case that resolves around the Eighth Amendments ban on cruel and unsual punishment in death penalty cases. The Furman v. Georgia case was based on Furman being convicted of murder in Georgia. Furman was caught i
One major ethical conflict that may arise in my investigation is the Responsibility to Client in both Standard 3 and Standard 4 of the Ethical Standards for Human Service Professionals (2015). Making sure we do not disclose information without consent ev
4. Identify two examples of real world problems that you have observed in your personal
Summary & Evaluation: Reference & 188. Academic Search Ultimate
Ethics
We can mention at least one example of how the violation of ethical standards can be prevented. Many organizations promote ethical self-regulation by creating moral codes to help direct their business activities
*DDB is used for the first three years
For example
The inbound logistics for William Instrument refer to purchase components from various electronic firms. During the purchase process William need to consider the quality and price of the components. In this case
4. A U.S. Supreme Court case known as Furman v. Georgia (1972) is a landmark case that involved Eighth Amendment’s ban of unusual and cruel punishment in death penalty cases (Furman v. Georgia (1972)
With covid coming into place
In my opinion
with
Not necessarily all home buyers are the same! When you choose to work with we buy ugly houses Baltimore & nationwide USA
The ability to view ourselves from an unbiased perspective allows us to critically assess our personal strengths and weaknesses. This is an important step in the process of finding the right resources for our personal learning style. Ego and pride can be
· By Day 1 of this week
While you must form your answers to the questions below from our assigned reading material
CliftonLarsonAllen LLP (2013)
5 The family dynamic is awkward at first since the most outgoing and straight forward person in the family in Linda
Urien
The most important benefit of my statistical analysis would be the accuracy with which I interpret the data. The greatest obstacle
From a similar but larger point of view
4 In order to get the entire family to come back for another session I would suggest coming in on a day the restaurant is not open
When seeking to identify a patient’s health condition
After viewing the you tube videos on prayer
Your paper must be at least two pages in length (not counting the title and reference pages)
The word assimilate is negative to me. I believe everyone should learn about a country that they are going to live in. It doesnt mean that they have to believe that everything in America is better than where they came from. It means that they care enough
Data collection
Single Subject Chris is a social worker in a geriatric case management program located in a midsize Northeastern town. She has an MSW and is part of a team of case managers that likes to continuously improve on its practice. The team is currently using an
I would start off with Linda on repeating her options for the child and going over what she is feeling with each option. I would want to find out what she is afraid of. I would avoid asking her any “why” questions because I want her to be in the here an
Summarize the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psychological research (Comp 2.1) 25.0\% Summarization of the advantages and disadvantages of using an Internet site as means of collecting data for psych
Identify the type of research used in a chosen study
Compose a 1
Optics
effect relationship becomes more difficult—as the researcher cannot enact total control of another person even in an experimental environment. Social workers serve clients in highly complex real-world environments. Clients often implement recommended inte
I think knowing more about you will allow you to be able to choose the right resources
Be 4 pages in length
soft MB-920 dumps review and documentation and high-quality listing pdf MB-920 braindumps also recommended and approved by Microsoft experts. The practical test
g
One thing you will need to do in college is learn how to find and use references. References support your ideas. College-level work must be supported by research. You are expected to do that for this paper. You will research
Elaborate on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study 20.0\% Elaboration on any potential confounds or ethical concerns while participating in the psychological study is missing. Elaboration on any potenti
3 The first thing I would do in the family’s first session is develop a genogram of the family to get an idea of all the individuals who play a major role in Linda’s life. After establishing where each member is in relation to the family
A Health in All Policies approach
Note: The requirements outlined below correspond to the grading criteria in the scoring guide. At a minimum
Chen
Read Connecting Communities and Complexity: A Case Study in Creating the Conditions for Transformational Change
Read Reflections on Cultural Humility
Read A Basic Guide to ABCD Community Organizing
Use the bolded black section and sub-section titles below to organize your paper. For each section
Losinski forwarded the article on a priority basis to Mary Scott
Losinksi wanted details on use of the ED at CGH. He asked the administrative resident